Background

Despite the well-known clinical effects of midazolam and ketamine, including sedation and memory impairment, the neural mechanisms of these distinct drugs in humans are incompletely understood. The authors hypothesized that both drugs would decrease recollection memory, task-related brain activity, and long-range connectivity between components of the brain systems for memory encoding, pain processing, and fear learning.

Methods

In this randomized within-subject crossover study of 26 healthy adults, the authors used behavioral measures and functional magnetic resonance imaging to study these two anesthetics, at sedative doses, in an experimental memory paradigm using periodic pain. The primary outcome, recollection memory performance, was quantified with d′ (a difference of z scores between successful recognition versus false identifications). Secondary outcomes were familiarity memory performance, serial task response times, task-related brain responses, and underlying brain connectivity from 17 preselected anatomical seed regions. All measures were determined under saline and steady-state concentrations of the drugs.

Results

Recollection memory was reduced under midazolam (median [95% CI], d′ = 0.73 [0.43 to 1.02]) compared with saline (d′ = 1.78 [1.61 to 1.96]) and ketamine (d′ = 1.55 [1.12 to 1.97]; P < 0.0001). Task-related brain activity was detected under saline in areas involved in memory, pain, and fear, particularly the hippocampus, insula, and amygdala. Compared with saline, midazolam increased functional connectivity to 20 brain areas and decreased to 8, from seed regions in the precuneus, posterior cingulate, and left insula. Compared with saline, ketamine decreased connectivity to 17 brain areas and increased to 2, from 8 seed regions including the hippocampus, parahippocampus, amygdala, and anterior and primary somatosensory cortex.

Conclusions

Painful stimulation during light sedation with midazolam, but not ketamine, can be accompanied by increased coherence in brain connectivity, even though details are less likely to be recollected as explicit memories.

Editor’s Perspective
What We Already Know about This Topic
  • Light sedation with general anesthetics is known to decrease recollection of conscious painful experiences

  • Pharmacologically distinct anesthetic drugs differentially affect pain perception and memory encoding at varying levels of sedation

  • The neural correlates underlying these drug-specific differential effects are incompletely understood

What This Article Tells Us That Is New
  • In this randomized within-subject crossover study of healthy volunteers using an experimental memory paradigm using periodic pain under light sedation, recollection memory was reduced by midazolam compared with saline and ketamine

  • The paradigm-related brain activity differed between the two drugs: Whereas midazolam mostly increased, ketamine predominantly decreased functional connectivity from brain regions involved in memory encoding, pain processing, and threat response

  • These observations highlight how pharmacologically distinct general anesthetics may engage distinct neural dynamics to modulate cognitive experience under threat of pain

Memory formation and pain perception are among the cognitive functions fundamental to the human experience of consciousness. Anesthetic drugs are routinely used to modulate these and other elements of consciousness during what would otherwise be intolerably painful experiences. However, the neural correlates of anesthetic action in humans are incompletely understood. Comparative studies have demonstrated how pharmacologically distinct anesthetic agents differentially affect pain perception1–3  and memory encoding at varying levels of sedation.4–6  However, these coarse behavioral measures are limited in revealing how different drugs affect underlying brain activity. Functional neuroimaging provides a unique tool to better understand the complex milieu of anesthetic action during the experience of pain.

We performed a within-subject comparative neuroimaging study of two commonly used anesthetic agents, midazolam and ketamine. These distinct drugs are well-known to have different effects on discrete aspects of cognition. Whereas midazolam provides anxiolysis and reversible anterograde amnesia, ketamine produces analgesia and a dissociated mental state. Both drugs are used at low doses to provide sedation during experiences that could otherwise be characterized as unpleasant, painful, or anxiety-provoking. The brain regions involved in memory formation, pain processing, and fear learning are engaged by these experiences, and their inhibition may be important in preventing psychologic sequelae. We attempted to experimentally model such an experience, using repeated, unpredictable painful stimulation and a task that allowed later quantification of successful memory encoding.

Functional neuroimaging can quantify and localize distinct features of brain activity that underlie drug-induced differences in mental state. The blood oxygen level– dependent effect has been used for decades as a reliable surrogate for localized neuronal activity induced by task performance.7  Additionally, low-frequency (less than 0.1 Hz) fluctuations of the signal reflect meaningful periodic changes in neuronal activity that can be observed at rest or in the context of task performance.8  Functional connectivity is a measure of the coherence in these fluctuations over time, reflecting neuronal communication.9  In this study, we analyzed both task-related activity and functional connectivity to determine the neurosignature of midazolam and ketamine during memory encoding and acute pain.

As the primary outcome in this study, we expected reductions in explicit memory with both drugs but that recollection would be more impacted than familiarity, with both assessed during next-day recognition. We hypothesized that midazolam would cause a greater reduction in recollection compared with ketamine. Neuroimaging measures of brain activity and connectivity were evaluated as secondary outcomes. We predicted that, compared with saline, fewer task-related changes would be detected under both anesthetic agents in the brain regions associated with memory, pain, and fear processing. It was less clear how the drugs would impact functional connectivity between brain regions in the setting of experimental pain, but we anticipated reductions in long-range functional connectivity for both agents.

Study Design and Oversight

This was a randomized, single-blind, within-subject crossover neuroimaging trial comparing the effects of two anesthetics, midazolam and ketamine. A study flowchart is shown in figure 1. Written informed consent was obtained in person, at the first study visit, after a full discussion of risks and benefits. The study was approved by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board (PRO 14050609) and conformed to all relevant standards for the ethical and responsible conduct of research. The trial was prospectively registered with clinicaltrials.gov (NCT02515890).

Participants

Healthy volunteer participants between the ages of 18 and 39 yr were recruited from the community and compensated up to $200 for participation. Demographic information from all 26 participants is tabulated in table 1. All participants acknowledged being free from significant memory or hearing impairment, chronic pain, other chronic medical problems, and recent or regular use of antidepressants, antipsychotics, antihistamines, anxiolytics, stimulants, sleep aids, and analgesics. A preanesthetic evaluation by a study anesthesiologist, urine pregnancy test for females, and magnetic resonance imaging compatibility screening confirmed no other contraindications to safe sedation inside a high-field magnet. In addition to following American Society of Anesthesiologists (Schaumburg, Illinois) guidelines for fasting,10  participants abstained from tobacco and caffeine for 8 h before the magnetic resonance imaging sessions.

Painful Stimulation and Monitoring

An electric nerve stimulator (EzStim II; Life Tech, USA) was employed using a 100-Hz tetanic stimulation waveform. The stimulator was connected to electrodes on the left index finger, and current was slowly titrated to a subjective rating of 7 of 10 pain. The numerical rating scale for pain had anchors at 0 being no pain and 10 being the worst imaginable. Two 1-s test shocks were delivered once in the scanner, and a pain rating was obtained, as listed in table 2. The stimulator was readjusted at that time, if necessary, but not further manipulated during the experiment. Pain scores and any subjective complaints were obtained from participants and recorded at the end of the saline segment and after each of the three blocks in the drug segment (timing is diagrammed in fig. 2).

Infusions

Intravenous access was obtained in each participant’s right hand with a 22-gauge catheter. Once positioned in the magnetic resonance imaging scanner and connected to standard American Society of Anesthesiologists monitors, a saline carrier infusion was run at 75 ml/h until the end of the experiment. After completion of tasks during the saline control condition, drug administration was started. Pre-experimental computer simulation was used to determine the optimal bolus and infusion doses to efficiently achieve and maintain steady-state drug concentrations, using the open-source software STANPUMP (Stanford University, USA [http://opentci.org/code/stanpump; accessed April 3, 2021]). Brain effect site concentrations of 10 ng/ml for midazolam and 200 ng/ml for ketamine were targeted, using pharmacokinetic models11,12  that accounted for age, sex, height, and weight. The total drug dose administered to each participant is listed in table 1.

Memory Encoding Task

The experiment was implemented with E-Prime version 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, USA). Shock delivery synchronization to follow experimental word items was accomplished with E-Prime control of custom-built hardware. A schematic timeline is shown in figure 2. Participants made category judgments about each word’s meaning (Alive or not?) and responded by pressing a button with their right index (yes) or middle finger (no).13  Ninety words were used in each segment, 30 of which were immediately followed by a 1-s electric shock. No more than two pain-paired words nor five non-pain words occurred consecutively. Word order was randomized between repetition blocks, although pain pairing was kept consistent. To increase statistical power for detecting task events in the imaging data, 0- to 6-s periods of jitter were included between items.14 

Response Times

During the encoding segments, the participant response window began at the start of the word being played and closed after 6 s. Response time outliers were removed using RStudio (USA; version 1.0.153 [https://www.rstudio.com/; accessed April 3, 2021]) running R version 3.2.5. The median absolute deviation was calculated,15  and response time values greater than 3.5 times the median absolute deviation from the grand median (across subjects) were defined as outliers. Outliers represented less than 1% of all data.

Memory Testing

Explicit memory testing occurred the next day, 20 to 32 h after the scanning session. Recognition testing used the Remember-Know-New scheme16  (subject instructions were published previously13 ). Recollection (of specific details) was indicated by a Remember response, whereas a Know response indicated familiarity (recognized, but with no specific details recollected). Participants were instructed to mark the word as New if they did not recognize it. All words heard the previous day were played intermixed with an equal number of foils, in randomized order. As in previous similar studies,17  memory performance was summarized using the signal detection metric, d′,18  which is calculated from the difference in z scores between cumulative Gaussian distributions: z(hits) – z(false alarms); hits are correctly recognized previously heard items, and false alarms are foils incorrectly identified with Remember or Know responses. This more rigorous metric accounts for subjects’ false alarm rate, in a way that correct responses alone would not. As an example, a 98% hit rate with 98% false alarms would yield a d′ of zero, reflecting the inability to discriminate between previously heard and unheard words. Thus, measures of both recollection and familiarity were determined separately and treated as distinct components of long-term explicit memory.

Magnetic Resonance Imaging

Imaging was performed on a Siemens (USA) Prisma 3 T scanner using a 32-channel head coil. Functional images were obtained using a gradient-echo echo-planar imaging sequence, with the following parameters: echo time = 30 ms, repetition time = 1 s, flip angle = 45°, bandwidth 2,004 Hz/pixel, and anterior-posterior phase encoding. Sixty slices at 2.3 mm isotropic spatial resolution gave whole brain coverage (including the cerebellum), and these were obtained in interleaved fashion using multiband acceleration (factor 5). For subsequent correction of magnetic field in homogeneities, a gradient echo field map was acquired after each functional imaging run in the same coordinate space. For later registration, a T1-weighted anatomical image was obtained with 1 mm isotropic spatial resolution.

Statistical Analysis

The primary outcome, recollection memory performance, was evaluated by comparing d′ scores (where d′ represents a difference of z scores between successful recognition versus false identification). Statistical analysis of response time data was carried out in SPSS Statistics 26 (IBM, USA). The common logarithm (log10) transformed the data to a normal distribution, and a linear mixed model with autoregressive covariance structure was used. Occurrence number, drug condition, and pain association were included as repeated fixed factors, including interaction terms. Data for both recollection and familiarity performance failed tests for normality, so statistical comparisons for d′ were performed with independent-sample Kruskal–Wallis tests in SPSS. Recollection and familiarity data were analyzed separately and not compared with one another. All statistical analyses were two-tailed; reported P values are Bonferroni-adjusted for the number of comparisons, with P < 0.05 used as the threshold for significance. A power analysis for the primary outcome, based on extrapolations from pilot memory performance data, indicated that 16 participants would be needed to detect a 50% decrement in recollection performance with 80% power. No statistical power calculation was possible for the imaging analyses, but overall sample size was based on general estimates19,20  suggesting that 24 subjects are adequate for task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging studies.

In addition to the eight subjects who did not return for their second set of experimental sessions, unrecoverable scanner errors corrupted the imaging data from two subject sessions, which were not able to be included in the analysis. Functional images were preprocessed using Statistical Parameter Mapping software (University College London, United Kingdom; SPM12 v7219 [http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/; accessed April 3, 2021]) using a Windows 10 computer running MATLAB 2018b (Mathworks, USA). Initial steps included motion correction and unwarping (using the acquired field inhomogeneity maps) of the functional imaging data and tissue segmentation of the anatomical image. Excessive motion precluded use for 10 of the 168 independent functional datasets acquired. Before further analysis, the CompCor algorithm21  was used to reduce physiologic noise related to respiration and cardiac pulsatility. Thus, the principal components (first five Eigenseries) derived from the average white matter and cerebrospinal fluid signal timecourses were included as covariates of no interest, along with the motion parameters. Spatial smoothing was performed with a 5-mm Gaussian kernel. Event-related task activation was calculated, using an event window that started with onset of the word audio being played and ended once the participant had made their response. Four subject-level contrasts were included: all experimental items, items subsequently recollected (correct Remember responses), items subsequently recognized as familiar (correct Know responses), and all pain-paired items. Interactions between item types (correct Remember responses for pain-paired items) were not analyzed, because the number of available observations would be small for some subgroupings. Group average task activation maps were generated by averaging data within a drug condition. Voxels were initially thresholded for significance at P < 0.001, and then a cluster-determining threshold was applied that adjusted the family-wise error rate to P < 0.05.

Connectivity analyses were performed using Conn Toolbox22  version 18b (Massachusetts Institute of Technology, USA [https://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn/; accessed April 3, 2021]). As part of data preprocessing, Artifact Removal Tool (Conn Toolbox)–based outlier detection was used. Data were band-pass filtered (0.008 to 0.09 Hz); linear detrending was applied. A seed-to-voxel analysis was used, with seed regions defined in the Harvard-Oxford atlas available in Conn Toolbox. Seventeen seed regions were chosen, for hypothesized roles in memory, pain, or fear networks. These were (bilateral): anterior and posterior cingulate, amygdala, anterior and posterior parahippocampus, hippocampus, insula, precuneus, primary somatosensory cortex, and thalamus. In addition to CompCor and motion parameters, timing of all experimental events was included as a covariate of no interest, essentially removing task responses from the signal timecourse. This allows background connectivity to be assessed with minimal contamination from task events.23,24  Group-level connectivity contrasts for saline greater than midazolam and saline greater than ketamine were calculated and thresholded within Conn Toolbox, correcting the overall significance for a cluster false-discovery rate of P < 0.05.25  Complete lists of all clusters showing statistically significant connectivity change with drug are available (see tables, Supplemental Digital Content 1 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C585] for saline greater than midazolam, and Supplemental Digital Content 2 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C586] for saline greater than ketamine). Connectivity changes are summarized in tables 3 and 4, where identified target regions are organized by network in which a putative role may be assigned (such as the insula for pain processing26 ). All statistically significant connectivity changes are represented in the summary tables, but functionally related target areas (such as the left ventrolateral and the right and left dorsolateral prefrontal regions) are collapsed into a single row. Based on manual inspection of the data, functional neuroscience labels were assigned in the summary tables, in place of anatomical atlas labels, such as ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, rather than superior frontal gyrus.

Behavioral Measures

Pain scores (listed in table 2) from the encoding trial segments performed during drug infusion were significantly reduced under ketamine (median decrease of 1.6 on a 0 to 10 numerical rating scale; P < 0.001) but not midazolam (P = 0.194). Data noted as missing in table 2 also reflect that eight subjects did not return for their second drug session. Subject reports attributed to dissociation or dysphoria were recorded in 6 of 22 ketamine sessions (and no midazolam sessions). Subject reports of sedation were recorded in 4 of 22 ketamine and 8 of 22 midazolam sessions.

Response times for the categorization decision task performed in the scanner were not statistically different between drugs (P = 0.065), suggesting equal psychomotor impairment (fig. 3). Items with no recorded response were infrequent (449/23,220), at fewer than 2% of all trials (see table, Supplemental Digital Content 3 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C587], for individual response rates), demonstrating that participants maintained voluntary motor responsiveness to verbal cues.

Individual hit rates tabulated according to experimental conditions are available for review (see table, Supplemental Digital Content 4 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C588], for saline; Supplemental Digital Content 5 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C589], for midazolam; Supplemental Digital Content 6 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C590], for ketamine). False alarm responses were also tabulated (see table, Supplemental Digital Content 7 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C591]). Figure 4 displays the results for recollection and familiarity from next-day recognition testing, using the summary statistic d′. Consistent with previous results13  demonstrating that painful stimulations may affect memory for non-pain items within the same experimental block, no differences between pain-paired versus non-pain items were detected (P = 0.813; fig. 4A). Collapsing across pain-pairing (fig. 4B), median recollection under saline was d′ = 1.78 (95% CI, 1.61 to 1.96). Recollection performance under midazolam (d′ = 0.73 [0.43 to 1.02]) was significantly reduced compared with saline (P < 0.0001) and ketamine (d′ = 1.55 [1.12 to 1.97]; P < 0.001), representing a meaningful performance difference of more than 0.75 SD units. Comparisons of recollection between saline (d′ = 1.78 [1.61 to1.96]) and ketamine (d′ = 1.55 [1.12 to 1.97]) were not considered different (P = 0.081). Aggregate performance for familiarity, indicated by correct Know responses, did not significantly differ between saline (d′ = 0.36 [0.18 to 0.53]), midazolam (d′ = 0.31 [0.03 to 0.59]), and ketamine (d′ = 0.31 [0.02 to 0.61]), with overall P = 0.540. This predominance of impact on recollection has been previously shown for midazolam, which interferes with binding of words to their experimental context.27 

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Task-related Activation

A whole-brain annotated group average functional magnetic resonance imaging activation map is shown in figure 5, analyzed for event-related signal changes for items correctly recognized as familiar, based on the subsequent memory analysis. This represents a characteristic example of task-related activation, as similar maps were seen for correctly recollected items (see figure, Supplemental Digital Content 8 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C592]) and items specifically paired with painful stimulation (see figure, Supplemental Digital Content 9 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C593]). Note that all these maps represent average activation within the saline or drug conditions for one type of experimental item.

Statistically significant task-related brain activity under saline was seen in specific areas within different functional networks. These include primary memory encoding areas: the hippocampus, parahippocampus, and temporal gyrus (predominantly right-sided). Activation also occurred in the left amygdala, dorsal (or mid-) anterior cingulate, and bilateral insula. Activity in somatosensory network structures included the left primary and bilateral secondary somatosensory cortices, and the bilateral thalamus and brain stem. Motor network activation was seen in the cerebellum, supplementary motor area, and left primary motor cortex. Task-related activity was also detected in the left ventrolateral and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortices, in the lateral and medial parietal association areas, and in the posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus.

Figure 5 (and figures in Supplemental Digital Content 8 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C592] and 9 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C593]) also shows task activation for experimental items experienced under both drugs. It is important to note that the visual differences between columns in these figures are not definitive; when analyzed as a saline versus drug contrast, no clusters survive thresholding (analyses not shown).

Functional Connectivity Changes with Drug Administration

Of the 17 seed regions investigated, only the precuneus, posterior cingulate, and left insula showed significant changes in background connectivity under midazolam. Table 3 summarizes the 28 independent changes in connectivity seen under midazolam (for a complete list of clusters and coordinates of local maxima, see the table in Supplemental Digital Content 1 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C585]). The direction of change was predominantly increased in these data (20 of 28 statistically significant changes detected), which were acquired during task performance and thus the periodic experience of pain. Increases in background connectivity included distant targets identified across frontal, temporal, occipital, and parietal lobes. Although they were the minority of changes, decreases in long-range connectivity were also seen with midazolam. Characteristic examples include decreases from the posterior cingulate to bilateral targets in the anterior cingulate and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex.

With ketamine administration in this experimental paradigm, statistically significant changes in background connectivity were detected from 8 seed regions to 19 independent target clusters throughout the brain, summarized in table 4 (complete list of clusters and coordinates of local maxima in table, Supplemental Digital Content 2 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C586]). Two changes with ketamine were increases in connectivity: from the anterior cingulate to left medial frontal lobe and from the left posterior cingulate to left lateral parietal lobe. The remaining 17 changes under ketamine were decreases in functional connectivity, which occurred both between and within the networks for memory, pain, and fear. Notable between-systems decreases in connectivity were from the right amygdala to the right primary somatosensory cortex and from the right hippocampus to the right primary somatosensory cortex. Additionally, ketamine disrupted connectivity between the seed regions investigated and prefrontal and parietal areas (both targets identified as having decreased connectivity from the hippocampus). Finally, decreased connectivity was detected from the posterior cingulate to targets identified in the hippocampus and anterior cingulate.

We present a comparative neuroimaging study of two distinct anesthetic agents. Participants encoded words heard while periodically experiencing painful shock, which we expected to activate somatosensory and pain processing areas, as well as fear learning centers. The task-related activity under saline showed functional magnetic resonance imaging activation across many brain areas involved in these cognitive processes directly related to task response. Specifically, activations in the hippocampus, parahippocampus, and temporal gyrus, in combination with the lateral prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex, represent the core memory network.28,29  Activations in the amygdala and anterior cingulate demonstrate engagement of core elements in fear response.30–33  These function in a larger network involved in classical conditioning that include the hippocampus,34  parahippocampus,35  and connections to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex.34  The somatosensory network, which most prominently features the thalamus and primary and secondary somatosensory cortices, is commonly activated in pain processing.36  Activity seen in the insula represents a structure with specificity for pain processing,36  although the insula is also active in fear conditioning.30,33  Additional activation in the posterior cingulate and precuneus (core components in the default-mode network37–39 ) as well as cognitive processing areas in the prefrontal and parietal cortices likely reflect task-related attention shifts and higher-level cortical integration of the task experiences.

The recurring and unpredictable pattern of experiencing pain likely induced a state of threat during the entire experiment. This is supported by insula and somatosensory cortex activation in the saline average maps for the correctly recognized items (see fig. 5 for familiarity and figure, Supplemental Digital Content 8 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C592], for recollection) even though only approximately one third of these items were paired with a painful shock. In fact, the map for successfully recognized items closely matches the average map for all pain-paired items (see figure, Supplemental Digital Content 9 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C593]). A relatively larger magnitude of signal change after pain-paired items relative to non-pain items could explain some of this similarity in the calculated maps. However, another possibility is that the pain experience affects brain activation after nonpainful experimental trials as well. Behavioral evidence from a similarly designed experiment supports this notion, as memory for non-pain items were influenced by the presence of pain-paired items in the same experimental context.13 

Signal changes in the hippocampus, parahippocampus, and amygdala did not surpass the threshold for significance in the average task activation maps for either drug. This suggests (but is not direct evidence of) less task-related neuronal activity under both drugs. This nondefinitive result was not supported by a significant saline versus drug contrast but may inform the design of future studies. Human neuroimaging evidence for the effect of midazolam on medial temporal lobe memory structures is unreported,40  but previous qualitative results show reduced hippocampal activity under propofol in a similar paradigm.17  Previous task-based functional studies under ketamine have shown reduced activation related to memory41  and pain42  tasks.

After regression of task-related signal changes, midazolam and ketamine had different effects on underlying brain functional connectivity during the experience of pain. Changes in background connectivity cut across functional networks, as targets were identified across all brain lobes, suggesting effects on higher-level integration of information. These cross-network changes in functional connectivity are exemplified well by the changes seen from the insula seed region under midazolam. The insula, with known specificity for pain processing,26  showed increases in connectivity to the anterior cingulate (involved in pain processing and fear learning), the middle temporal gyrus (involved in higher-order memory processing), and prefrontal cortex (involved in higher-level cognitive processing).

Compared with saline, midazolam significantly altered connectivity from three seed regions identified in our analysis, and these were within the pain (insula) and default-mode (precuneus and posterior cingulate) networks. The direction of connectivity change was mixed with midazolam, but we found several decreases in long-range connectivity (such as from the posterior cingulate seed to prefrontal and anterior cingulate targets). This aligns with previous work (though a small study using older scanner technology and higher doses of midazolam) that showed reduced connectivity from the posterior cingulate to other areas.43  In contrast, our findings of predominantly increased connectivity with midazolam diverge from some previous resting-state studies. A recent study, which showed reduced connectivity across many predefined brain networks, used a dose (2 mg) of midazolam comparable with our study. However, this was done in a smaller number of much older subjects, ranging from 55 to 73 yr.44  Our midazolam connectivity findings align best with a previous study45  that used a similar dose administered as a bolus 5 min before a 7-min-long resting-state scan. Although liberally thresholded (without a correction for multiple comparisons), increases in connectivity with midazolam were seen in sensorimotor networks bilaterally and in some portions of networks identified as “language” (anterior insula and temporal gyrus), “frontoparietal” (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate), and “salience” (dorsal anterior cingulate and left anterior insula).45  In the same study, decreased connectivity under midazolam was found for other networks investigated.45  With our finding of predominantly increased connectivity during a pain task and midazolam, it is important to note that low doses of midazolam can be disinhibitory.46  Further, increases in connectivity between functionally distinct brain regions have been demonstrated with sedative doses of other anesthetics that similarly act as γ-aminobutyric acid receptor agonists.47–49 

Under ketamine, connectivity changes between brain regions within our networks of interest were predominantly decreased, compared with saline. Between-systems decreases in connectivity under ketamine were notable from fear learning (right amygdala) and memory encoding (right hippocampus) areas to the right primary somatosensory cortex. Noting that the painful stimulation was left-sided, this finding suggests a disruption of the link between these two learning systems and brain region primarily processing the painful stimulus. Additionally, ketamine disrupted connectivity between the seed regions investigated and cortical areas involved in higher-level processing, such as prefrontal and parietal association areas; both were targets identified as having decreased connectivity from the hippocampus. Finally, decreased connectivity was detected from the posterior cingulate within the default-mode network (involved in attention) to targets identified in the hippocampus (memory) and anterior cingulate (fear learning and pain processing).

Previous ketamine connectivity findings50  showed decreases in connectivity between a large somatosensory seed network (including the insula) and target regions in the amygdala, anterior cingulate, and hippocampus, consistent with the pattern we detected. Within-network connectivity decreases with ketamine51  can be seen, as can relative increases in default-mode network connectivity at higher doses.52  It should be noted that variability across studies in choosing how to group anatomically disparate seed and target regions of interest for connectivity analyses makes comparison with previous work not straightforward. It is our hope that granular reporting of connectivity changes for individual anatomically described regions of interest helps to resolve this issue in the future.

This study has important limitations. The use of a single drug dose limits the generalizability of our findings. Although there would be great value in collecting behavioral and imaging data in a similar paradigm across a range of drug dose responses, this was beyond the scope of the current project. Doses were chosen to target roughly equal sedation at a level that maintained some explicit memory ability, to allow analysis of neural activity of successfully recognized items. However, subjects did have considerable variability in drug effects. Poignant examples of interindividual pharmacodynamic differences include analgesic response to ketamine (table 2) and amnestic response from midazolam (compare tables in Supplemental Digital Content 4 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C588] and 5 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C589]). These differences in individual drug responsiveness capture population variability but decrease the power in our saline versus drug imaging comparisons. Also, the presence of fewer accurately recognized items under both drugs influenced the power of the activation maps for recollection (see figure, Supplemental Digital Content 8 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C592]). This concern is ameliorated, however, by the similar numbers of saline and drug experimental items analyzed in the familiarity (fig. 5) and pain-paired (see figure, Supplemental Digital Content 9 [https://links.lww.com/ALN/C593]) maps. Another consideration is that activation was averaged over the three repetitions of the word. It is possible that different strengths of signal change occurred with each repetition of the word, and our analysis framework looks for changes in the average neural activity over these repetitions. Differing levels of neural activity with each repetition may modestly decrease the effect size observed for the activation results. Finally, the possibility of implicit memory was not addressed by our paradigm. We have previously shown, in an unsedated non–magnetic resonance imaging cohort, that our pain stimulation paradigm causes robust skin responses with shock delivery,53  but this recording was not possible in the scanner. Further work in this area, using a more typical conditioning paradigm, could clarify whether learned sympathetic responses are modulated by these anesthetics.

In summary, in a clinically relevant paradigm of memory formation with periodic pain, low doses of midazolam reduced recollection memory. Midazolam and ketamine were associated with diverging functional connectivity changes, which we postulate reflects their differing activity in the modulation of the cognitive experience under threat of pain. This snapshot of the complex neural dynamics from the combined effects of pain and anesthesia underscores the need to better understand residual brain activity during sedation, particularly in the setting of distinct agents that differentially affect specific cognitive functions.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful for the critical comments and helpful suggestions from the handling editor and anonymous reviewers, who helped to sharpen this work into the current form, which is greatly improved from its initial version.

Research Support

This work was supported by the Department of Anesthesiology, University of Pittsburgh, School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania. Significant research funding and salary support was provided by a Mentored Research Training Grant (to Dr. Vogt) from the Foundation for Anesthesia Education and Research (Schaumburg, Illinois; MRTG-CT 2-2017). Salary support for Dr. Vogt during early development work directly related to this project was supported by an institutional training grant (T32GM075770), and support during data analysis and manuscript preparation came from a career development award (K23GM132755), both from the U.S. National Institutes of Health (Bethesda, Maryland), specifically the National Institute of General Medical Sciences (Bethesda, Maryland). Additional financial support for Dr. Vogt came from the National Institutes of Health Clinical Loan Repayment program (L30GM120759). Subject recruitment was assisted by the University of Pittsburgh Clinical Translational Science Institute Research Participant Registry, a project supported by the National Institutes of Health (UL1TR000005).

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

1.
Angst
MS
,
Ramaswamy
B
,
Davies
MF
,
Maze
M
:
Comparative analgesic and mental effects of increasing plasma concentrations of dexmedetomidine and alfentanil in humans.
Anesthesiology
.
2004
;
101
:
744
52
2.
Schulte
H
,
Graven-Nielsen
T
,
Sollevi
A
,
Jansson
Y
,
Arendt-Nielsen
L
,
Segerdahl
M
:
Pharmacological modulation of experimental phasic and tonic muscle pain by morphine, alfentanil and ketamine in healthy volunteers.
Acta Anaesthesiol Scand
.
2003
;
47
:
1020
30
3.
Frölich
MA
,
Zhang
K
,
Ness
TJ
:
Effect of sedation on pain perception.
Anesthesiology
.
2013
;
118
:
611
21
4.
Veselis
RA
,
Reinsel
RA
,
Feshchenko
VA
,
Wroński
M
:
The comparative amnestic effects of midazolam, propofol, thiopental, and fentanyl at equisedative concentrations.
Anesthesiology
.
1997
;
87
:
749
64
5.
Veselis
RA
,
Pryor
KO
,
Reinsel
RA
,
Li
Y
,
Mehta
M
,
Johnson
R
Jr
:
Propofol and midazolam inhibit conscious memory processes very soon after encoding: An event-related potential study of familiarity and recollection in volunteers.
Anesthesiology
.
2009
;
110
:
295
312
6.
Hetem
LA
,
Danion
JM
,
Diemunsch
P
,
Brandt
C
:
Effect of a subanesthetic dose of ketamine on memory and conscious awareness in healthy volunteers.
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
.
2000
;
152
:
283
8
7.
Rosen
BR
,
Savoy
RL
:
fMRI at 20: Has it changed the world?
Neuroimage
.
2012
;
62
:
1316
24
8.
Keilholz
SD
:
The neural basis of time-varying resting-state functional connectivity.
Brain Connect
.
2014
;
4
:
769
79
9.
Zhang
X
,
Pan
WJ
,
Keilholz
SD
:
The relationship between BOLD and neural activity arises from temporally sparse events.
Neuroimage
.
2020
;
207
:
116390
10.
ASA
:
Practice guidelines for preoperative fasting and the use of pharmacologic agents to reduce the risk of pulmonary aspiration: Application to healthy patients undergoing elective procedures: An updated report by the American Society of Anesthesiologists Task Force on Preoperative Fasting and the Use of Pharmacologic Agents to Reduce the Risk of Pulmonary Aspiration.
Anesthesiology
.
2017
;
126
:
376
93
11.
Greenblatt
DJ
,
Ehrenberg
BL
,
Gunderman
J
,
Locniskar
A
,
Scavone
JM
,
Harmatz
JS
,
Shader
RI
:
Pharmacokinetic and electroencephalographic study of intravenous diazepam, midazolam, and placebo.
Clin Pharmacol Ther
.
1989
;
45
:
356
65
12.
Domino
EF
,
Domino
SE
,
Smith
RE
,
Domino
LE
,
Goulet
JR
,
Domino
KE
,
Zsigmond
EK
:
Ketamine kinetics in unmedicated and diazepam-premedicated subjects.
Clin Pharmacol Ther
.
1984
;
36
:
645
53
13.
Vogt
KM
,
Norton
CM
,
Speer
LE
,
Tremel
JJ
,
Ibinson
JW
,
Reder
LM
,
Fiez
JA
:
Memory for non-painful auditory items is influenced by whether they are experienced in a context involving painful electrical stimulation.
Exp Brain Res
.
2019
;
237
:
1615
27
14.
Amaro
E
Jr
,
Barker
GJ
:
Study design in fMRI: Basic principles.
Brain Cogn
.
2006
;
60
:
220
32
15.
Leys
C
,
Ley
C
,
Klein
O
,
Bernard
P
,
Licata
L
:
Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median.
J Exp Soc Psychol
.
2013
;
49
:
764
6
16.
Migo
EM
,
Mayes
AR
,
Montaldi
D
:
Measuring recollection and familiarity: Improving the remember/know procedure.
Conscious Cogn
.
2012
;
21
:
1435
55
17.
Pryor
KO
,
Root
JC
,
Mehta
M
,
Stern
E
,
Pan
H
,
Veselis
RA
,
Silbersweig
DA
:
Effect of propofol on the medial temporal lobe emotional memory system: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in human subjects.
Br J Anaesth
.
2015
;
115
(
suppl 1
):
i104
13
18.
Stanislaw
H
,
Todorov
N
:
Calculation of signal detection theory measures.
Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput
.
1999
;
31
:
137
49
19.
Desmond
JE
,
Glover
GH
:
Estimating sample size in functional MRI (fMRI) neuroimaging studies: Statistical power analyses.
J Neurosci Methods
.
2002
;
118
:
115
28
20.
Mumford
JA
,
Nichols
TE
:
Power calculation for group fMRI studies accounting for arbitrary design and temporal autocorrelation.
Neuroimage
.
2008
;
39
:
261
8
21.
Behzadi
Y
,
Restom
K
,
Liau
J
,
Liu
TT
:
A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI.
Neuroimage
.
2007
;
37
:
90
101
22.
Whitfield-Gabrieli
S
,
Nieto-Castanon
A
:
Conn: A functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks.
Brain Connect
.
2012
;
2
:
125
41
23.
Elkhetali
AS
,
Fleming
LL
,
Vaden
RJ
,
Nenert
R
,
Mendle
JE
,
Visscher
KM
:
Background connectivity between frontal and sensory cortex depends on task state, independent of stimulus modality.
Neuroimage
.
2019
;
184
:
790
800
24.
Tompary
A
,
Al-Aidroos
N
,
Turk-Browne
NB
:
Attending to what and where: Background connectivity integrates categorical and spatial attention.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
2018
;
30
:
1281
97
25.
Chumbley
J
,
Worsley
K
,
Flandin
G
,
Friston
K
:
Topological FDR for neuroimaging.
Neuroimage
.
2010
;
49
:
3057
64
26.
Vogt
KM
,
Becker
CJ
,
Wasan
AD
,
Ibinson
JW
:
Human posterior insula functional connectivity differs between electrical pain and the resting state.
Brain Connect
.
2016
;
6
:
786
94
27.
Reder
LM
,
Proctor
I
,
Anderson
JR
,
Gyulai
F
,
Quinlan
JJ
,
Oates
JM
:
Midazolam does not inhibit association formation, just its storage and strengthening.
Psychopharmacology (Berl)
.
2006
;
188
:
462
71
28.
Squire
LR
,
Dede
AJ
:
Conscious and unconscious memory systems.
Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol
.
2015
;
7
:
a021667
29.
Kim
H
:
Neural activity that predicts subsequent memory and forgetting: A meta-analysis of 74 fMRI studies.
Neuroimage
.
2011
;
54
:
2446
61
30.
Visser
RM
,
Scholte
HS
,
Beemsterboer
T
,
Kindt
M
:
Neural pattern similarity predicts long-term fear memory.
Nat Neurosci
.
2013
;
16
:
388
90
31.
Milad
MR
,
Quirk
GJ
,
Pitman
RK
,
Orr
SP
,
Fischl
B
,
Rauch
SL
:
A role for the human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in fear expression.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2007
;
62
:
1191
4
32.
Maier
S
,
Szalkowski
A
,
Kamphausen
S
,
Perlov
E
,
Feige
B
,
Blechert
J
,
Philipsen
A
,
van Elst
LT
,
Kalisch
R
,
Tüscher
O
:
Clarifying the role of the rostral dmPFC/dACC in fear/anxiety: Learning, appraisal or expression?
PLoS One
.
2012
;
7
:
e50120
33.
van Well
S
,
Visser
RM
,
Scholte
HS
,
Kindt
M
:
Neural substrates of individual differences in human fear learning: Evidence from concurrent fMRI, fear-potentiated startle, and US-expectancy data.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
.
2012
;
12
:
499
512
34.
Izquierdo
I
,
Furini
CR
,
Myskiw
JC
:
Fear memory.
Physiol Rev
.
2016
;
96
:
695
750
35.
Reinhardt
I
,
Jansen
A
,
Kellermann
T
,
Schüppen
A
,
Kohn
N
,
Gerlach
AL
,
Kircher
T
:
Neural correlates of aversive conditioning: Development of a functional imaging paradigm for the investigation of anxiety disorders.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
.
2010
;
260
:
443
53
36.
Xu
A
,
Larsen
B
,
Baller
EB
,
Scott
JC
,
Sharma
V
,
Adebimpe
A
,
Basbaum
AI
,
Dworkin
RH
,
Edwards
RR
,
Woolf
CJ
,
Eickhoff
SB
,
Eickhoff
CR
,
Satterthwaite
TD
:
Convergent neural representations of experimentally-induced acute pain in healthy volunteers: A large-scale fMRI meta-analysis.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2020
;
112
:
300
23
37.
Fransson
P
,
Marrelec
G
:
The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis.
Neuroimage
.
2008
;
42
:
1178
84
38.
Leech
R
,
Kamourieh
S
,
Beckmann
CF
,
Sharp
DJ
:
Fractionating the default mode network: Distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control.
J Neurosci
.
2011
;
31
:
3217
24
39.
Utevsky
AV
,
Smith
DV
,
Huettel
SA
:
Precuneus is a functional core of the default-mode network.
J Neurosci
.
2014
;
34
:
932
40
40.
Liang
P
,
Manelis
A
,
Liu
X
,
Aizenstein
HJ
,
Gyulai
F
,
Quinlan
JJ
,
Reder
LM
:
Using arterial spin labeling perfusion MRI to explore how midazolam produces anterograde amnesia.
Neurosci Lett
.
2012
;
522
:
113
7
41.
Honey
GD
,
Honey
RA
,
O’Loughlin
C
,
Sharar
SR
,
Kumaran
D
,
Suckling
J
,
Menon
DK
,
Sleator
C
,
Bullmore
ET
,
Fletcher
PC
:
Ketamine disrupts frontal and hippocampal contribution to encoding and retrieval of episodic memory: An fMRI study.
Cereb Cortex
.
2005
;
15
:
749
59
42.
Sprenger
T
,
Valet
M
,
Woltmann
R
,
Zimmer
C
,
Freynhagen
R
,
Kochs
EF
,
Tölle
TR
,
Wagner
KJ
:
Imaging pain modulation by subanesthetic S-(+)-ketamine.
Anesth Analg
.
2006
;
103
:
729
37
43.
Greicius
MD
,
Kiviniemi
V
,
Tervonen
O
,
Vainionpää
V
,
Alahuhta
S
,
Reiss
AL
,
Menon
V
:
Persistent default-mode network connectivity during light sedation.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2008
;
29
:
839
47
44.
Frölich
MA
,
White
DM
,
Kraguljac
NV
,
Lahti
AC
:
Baseline functional connectivity predicts connectivity changes due to a small dose of midazolam in older adults.
Anesth Analg
.
2020
;
130
:
224
32
45.
Liang
P
,
Zhang
H
,
Xu
Y
,
Jia
W
,
Zang
Y
,
Li
K
:
Disruption of cortical integration during midazolam-induced light sedation.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2015
;
36
:
4247
61
46.
Ibrahim
AE
,
Ghoneim
MM
,
Kharasch
ED
,
Epstein
RH
,
Groudine
SB
,
Ebert
TJ
,
Binstock
WB
,
Philip
BK
;
Sevoflurane Sedation Study Group
:
Speed of recovery and side-effect profile of sevoflurane sedation compared with midazolam.
Anesthesiology
.
2001
;
94
:
87
94
47.
Liu
X
,
Li
SJ
,
Hudetz
AG
:
Increased precuneus connectivity during propofol sedation.
Neurosci Lett
.
2014
;
561
:
18
23
48.
Martuzzi
R
,
Ramani
R
,
Qiu
M
,
Shen
X
,
Papademetris
X
,
Constable
RT
:
A whole-brain voxel based measure of intrinsic connectivity contrast reveals local changes in tissue connectivity with anesthetic without a priori assumptions on thresholds or regions of interest.
Neuroimage
.
2011
;
58
:
1044
50
49.
Stamatakis
EA
,
Adapa
RM
,
Absalom
AR
,
Menon
DK
:
Changes in resting neural connectivity during propofol sedation.
PLoS One
.
2010
;
5
:
e14224
50.
Niesters
M
,
Khalili-Mahani
N
,
Martini
C
,
Aarts
L
,
van Gerven
J
,
van Buchem
MA
,
Dahan
A
,
Rombouts
S
:
Effect of subanesthetic ketamine on intrinsic functional brain connectivity: A placebo-controlled functional magnetic resonance imaging study in healthy male volunteers.
Anesthesiology
.
2012
;
117
:
868
77
51.
Adhikari
BM
,
Dukart
J
,
Hipp
JF
,
Forsyth
A
,
McMillan
R
,
Muthukumaraswamy
SD
,
Ryan
MC
,
Hong
LE
,
Eickhoff
SB
,
Jahandshad
N
,
Thompson
PM
,
Rowland
LM
,
Kochunov
P
:
Effects of ketamine and midazolam on resting state connectivity and comparison with ENIGMA connectivity deficit patterns in schizophrenia.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2020
;
41
:
767
78
52.
Bonhomme
V
,
Vanhaudenhuyse
A
,
Demertzi
A
,
Bruno
MA
,
Jaquet
O
,
Bahri
MA
,
Plenevaux
A
,
Boly
M
,
Boveroux
P
,
Soddu
A
,
Brichant
JF
,
Maquet
P
,
Laureys
S
:
Resting-state network-specific breakdown of functional connectivity during ketamine alteration of consciousness in volunteers.
Anesthesiology
.
2016
;
125
:
873
88
53.
Citro
AT
,
Norton
CM
,
Pcola
SJ
,
Vogt
KM
:
Psychometric and electrodermal activity data from an experimental paradigm of memory encoding with some items periodically followed by painful electric shock.
Data Brief
.
2020
;
31
:
105669