THE mechanism by which anesthetics impair consciousness remains one of the most important unsolved mysteries of our specialty—if not of all science. Establishing causal links connecting molecular sites of action with cellular, network, and ultimately behavioral changes will be a formidable challenge. One important approach that can contribute to this effort is the use of mathematical models that can synthesize detailed information about individual channels, cellular properties, and interconnections to yield predictions about the behaviors of complex systems. Experiments can be conducted in silico  to investigate how individual components contribute to network properties in a way that is difficult or impossible using in vitro  or in vivo  approaches. In this issue of Anesthesiology, Gottschalk and Miotke1make use of this computational approach to investigate how anesthetic modulation of two potentially important molecular targets, γ-aminobutyric acid type A (GABAA) receptors and T-type calcium channels, might contribute to changes in thalamocortical network properties during anesthesia, with the ultimate goal of explaining how anesthetics render us unconscious.

The target of their investigation is a network of inhibitory cells that comprise the reticular nucleus of the thalamus (RTN). The thalamus has long been recognized as the gateway to the cortex, with sensory information transmitted via  relay cells, the primary projection neurons of the thalamus, to arrive at primary sensory cortex. More recently, the thalamus has also been recognized to play an important, perhaps even dominant, role in the communication between different regions of the cortex via  indirect corticothalamocortical pathways.2The cells of the RTN provide an inhibitory input to the relay cells, so they are in an ideal position to control the flow of information to and through the cortex.3 

A remarkable feature of the activity of both RTN and thalamic relay cells is the presence of two distinct firing modes. During alert wakefulness, these neurons are steadily depolarized and respond to excitatory inputs with streams of unitary action potentials, allowing them to relatively faithfully transmit incoming signals in a relatively linear manner.2However, during certain network states, such as nonrapid eye movement sleep and anesthesia, they synchronize their own firing and that of their thalamic targets to produce prominent network oscillations that can be observed in the electroencephalogram as sleep spindles, transient bouts of activity that have a dominant frequency of approximately 10 Hz and that occur typically just after the up-state transition during slow wave oscillations of sleep and anesthesia.4Two important characteristics that endow them with this ability are (1) γ-aminobutyric acid–mediated inhibitory synaptic connections that RTN cells make with each other and (2) the presence of low-threshold “transient” T-type Ca2+channels, which produce a slow calcium spike on which bursts of action potentials ride. The well-recognized transition of electroencephalographic activity during anesthesia from low-amplitude desynchronized activity in the awake state to increasingly synchronous large-amplitude oscillations that mimic those occurring during natural sleep (and in fact may well use many of the same elements) provided the motivation for examining the contributions of anesthetic modulation of GABAAreceptors and T-type Ca2+channels to synchronous activity.

The model on which the current study is based was developed using detailed morphologic and electrophysiologic studies of these specific cells and their synapses.5It has been used successfully in the past to model spindle generation by the RTN6and to demonstrate the role of neuromodulators in switching between firing modes.7As demonstrated previously, the model produces a spindle-like rhythm even in the absence of simulated anesthetic effects, though the output is far from synchronous. At first glance, this drug-free characteristic of spindle-like activity seems at odds with a model that is meant to simulate the transition from consciousness to unconsciousness, which is more commonly associated with a transition from tonic to burst firing mode. However, in the quiet resting state, thalamic neurons can respond with bursts to sensory input,8and similarities have been noted between sleep spindles and the α rhythm, a prominent oscillation seen in the electroencephalogram during quiet wakefulness and that may be produced by a similar mechanism.9,10Therefore, although the model as implemented here does not reproduce the stereotypical transition from tonic to burst firing mode associated with the transition from awake to unconscious, it may nevertheless provide an approximation of quiet waking network activity.

An important outstanding question that is particularly applicable to inhaled anesthetics (which can influence essentially any given physiologic process provided that a high enough concentration is administered) is to identify relevant molecular targets and to determine how the typically small effects that are seen at low “sedating” concentrations combine to produce the net effects of these agents. The current study begins to address this question by simulating the effects of halothane and isoflurane on GABAAreceptors and T-type Ca2+channels, using previous reports of their effects in a variety of other systems as surrogates for effects on these specific synapses and channels. Both anesthetics increased network synchrony, thus mimicking qualitatively one of the fundamental characteristics of the electroencephalogram during anesthesia. Unfortunately, there are not yet any physiologic studies of the isolated bursting RTN network to which these findings may be compared directly. (In this regard, this modeling study may serve as a wake-up call to physiologists interested in this question, for it should provide a useful framework for interpreting detailed pharmacologic studies.) Nevertheless, an important outcome of the study is the demonstration that even the modest effects produced by anesthetic concentrations associated with unconsciousness—less than a 20% increase in the decay time constant of GABAAreceptors and a 5% reduction in T-type Ca2+channel currents—combine to produce substantial changes in network synchrony. Although the authors emphasize the similarity in coherence that is produced in the model by these two anesthetics at behaviorally comparable concentrations, it is difficult from these results alone to know whether this reflects a meaningful causal relation among channel modulation, network coherence, and behavioral state or whether it is simply a fortuitous coincidence.

It is interesting that similar changes in coherence can be produced by modulating either GABAAreceptors or T-type Ca2+channels in the model and that the two anesthetics can arrive at a similar point by way of different paths. Using the ability to selectively manipulate each target that the computational approach provides, Gottschalk and Miotke go on to test whether the substantial changes in synchrony that derive from these small anesthetic effects reflect “additive” or “synergistic” effects at the network level, and to more generally explore the effects of combined influences on two separate anesthetic targets using isobolographic analysis. This is a timely topic, as demonstrated by a recent series of experimental and theoretical articles exploring additivity, synergy, and their implications for anesthetic mechanisms.11–14Despite the common expectation that drugs acting at different sites will show synergy, a general rule that was borne out by studies of the interactions among many drug classes of intravenous and inhaled drugs used in anesthetic practice,14they found that the combined effects in the model were largely additive. Indeed, deviations from additivity were not only modest but could be either synergistic or antagonistic depending on the specific drug and simulated concentration. This finding fits well with a separate theoretical analysis demonstrating that additivity is expected when drug effects are produced by concentrations associated with low receptor occupancy.13 

As do all studies, the current one has clear limitations, as readily acknowledged by the authors (and not surprising given the relative simplicity of the model that was studied) and raises additional questions. If reduced T-type Ca2+channel activity enhances synchrony, and this change is instrumental in altering consciousness as proposed, why do other drugs that block T-type Ca2+channels, such as ethosuximide, not produce hypnosis or sedation? Perhaps anesthetic-induced changes in this specific rhythm contribute to other aspects of thalamocortical function, such as sensory gating, as demonstrated recently for control of visceral pain sensation.15Clearly the activity of the thalamus is dramatically altered by anesthetics, but is the thalamus itself a critical target of these drugs? Or does its change in activity reflect altered inputs from below, above, or both? Given the accumulating evidence that the state of “anesthesia” shares many essential mechanisms with natural sleep16and the well-established role of the thalamus in sleep and consciousness, further studies of anesthetic modulation of thalamic function that can clarify its role in producing this essential component of anesthesia are clearly warranted. Few fields of study are as fascinating (or as difficult) as unraveling the mysteries of the mind. But, the powerful combination of physiologic experimentation and computation provides a promising path forward. The authors are to be commended for bringing the sophisticated tools of computation, an essential approach in contemporary neuroscience, to bear on this question.

Department of Anesthesiology, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison, Wisconsin. rapearce@wisc.edu

1.
Gottschalk A, Miotke SA: Volatile anesthetic action in a computational model of the thalamic reticular nucleus. Anesthesiology 2009; 110:996– 1010
2.
Sherman SM, Guillery RW: The role of the thalamus in the flow of information to the cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2002; 357:1695–708
3.
Guillery RW, Sherman SM: Thalamic relay functions and their role in corticocortical communication: Generalizations from the visual system. Neuron 2002; 33:163–75
4.
Destexhe A, Hughes SW, Rudolph M, Crunelli V: Are corticothalamic “up” states fragments of wakefulness? Trends Neurosci 2007; 30:334–42
5.
Destexhe A, Bal T, McCormick DA, Sejnowski TJ: Ionic mechanisms underlying synchronized oscillations and propagating waves in a model of ferret thalamic slices. J Neurophysiol 1996; 76:2049–70
6.
Destexhe A, Contreras D, Sejnowski TJ, Steriade M: A model of spindle rhythmicity in the isolated thalamic reticular nucleus. J Neurophysiol 1994; 72:803–18
7.
Destexhe A, Contreras D, Sejnowski TJ, Steriade M: Modeling the control of reticular thalamic oscillations by neuromodulators. Neuroreport 1994; 5:2217–20
8.
Sherman SM: Tonic and burst firing: Dual modes of thalamocortical relay. Trends Neurosci 2001; 24:122–6
9.
Suffczynski P, Kalitzin S, Pfurtscheller G, Lopes da Silva FH: Computational model of thalamo-cortical networks: Dynamical control of alpha rhythms in relation to focal attention. Int J Psychophysiol 2001; 43:25–40
10.
Schreckenberger M, Lange-Asschenfeldt C, Lochmann M, Mann K, Siessmeier T, Buchholz HG, Bartenstein P, Grunder G: The thalamus as the generator and modulator of EEG alpha rhythm: A combined PET/EEG study with lorazepam challenge in humans. Neuroimage 2004; 22:637–44
11.
Eger EI II, Tang M, Liao M, Laster MJ, Solt K, Flood P, Jenkins A, Raines D, Hendrickx JF, Shafer SL, Yasumasa T, Sonner JM: Inhaled anesthetics do not combine to produce synergistic effects regarding minimum alveolar anesthetic concentration in rats. Anesth Analg 2008; 107:479–85
12.
Eger EI II, Raines DE, Shafer SL, Hemmings HC Jr, Sonner JM: Is a new paradigm needed to explain how inhaled anesthetics produce immobility? Anesth Analg 2008; 107:832–48
13.
Shafer SL, Hendrickx JF, Flood P, Sonner J, Eger EI II: Additivity versus  synergy: A theoretical analysis of implications for anesthetic mechanisms. Anesth Analg 2008; 107:507–24.
14.
Hendrickx JF, Eger EI II, Sonner JM, Shafer SL: Is synergy the rule? A review of anesthetic interactions producing hypnosis and immobility. Anesth Analg 2008; 107:494–506
15.
Cheong E, Lee S, Choi BJ, Sun M, Lee CJ, Shin HS: Tuning thalamic firing modes via  simultaneous modulation of T- and L-type Ca2+ channels controls pain sensory gating in the thalamus. J Neurosci 2008; 28:13331–40
16.
Franks NP: General anaesthesia: From molecular targets to neuronal pathways of sleep and arousal. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008; 9:370–86