REGIONAL anesthesia is commonly used for a variety of surgical procedures; however, it is also used as a treatment for chronic pain. The effects of nerve blocks on chronic pain often far outlast the normal duration of a regional anesthetic block.1This suggests that the mechanisms by which peripheral nerve blocks improve chronic pain involve neural processes beyond those directly affected by the anesthesia. In a recent study, Silva et al.  2explore the far-reaching effects of regional anesthesia in the preoperative setting to help explain the acute plasticity of the nervous system that occurs when afferent input is disrupted.

A growing amount of literature shows that immediate changes in central somatotopic representations occur after regional anesthesia.3,4These changes are similar to how receptive fields of neurons from neighboring regions encroach into deprived regions of somatosensory cortex after amputation or sensory deafferentation.5–7By using a motor imagery task, Silva et al.  2identified psychologic/functional correlates associated with observed acute cortical plasticity after regional anesthesia. During a mental imagery hand rotation task, patients were presented pictures of hands in various positions and were asked to state whether the hand in the picture was a right or a left hand. This task requires the subject to rotate the pictured hand in his or her mind from its presented orientation and involves highly similar neurologic processes as those that occur when actually executing a movement.8After infraclavicular brachial plexus block of one arm, patients displayed impaired performance on the mental hand rotation task in conjunction with the experience of illusory sensory and proprioceptive sensations.

Previous studies4,9have focused on acute plastic changes within primary somatosensory and motor cortices after reversible deafferentation by anesthesia and ischemic nerve block; however, impairment of motor imagery task performance indicates that higher-order regions of cortex are also affected.2Although primarily somatosensory information was blocked, cortical regions involved in motor processing were affected by anesthetic deafferentation because regions involved in processing motor actions are known to subserve mental imagery of motor function.8Mental imagery of a motor task involves multiple regions of cortex, some of which overlap with motor execution, including premotor cortex, supplementary motor area, and cerebellum; others are suggested to involve multisensory integration, including superior and inferior lobules of the posterior parietal cortex.10–13Thus, although observations of plasticity in the somatosensory cortex are known to occur after deafferentation, it is apparent that regional anesthesia induces changes in additional regions implicated in motor imagery.

Additional evidence of the remote effects of regional anesthesia lies within the observation of the ability for visual input to modulate deficits in motor imagery. Patients were initially tested with their anesthetized arm hidden by a screen; however, in additional trials, when the anesthetized limb was in view, the deficits caused by anesthetic deafferentation were almost fully ameliorated (patients' reaction times and accuracy rates were practically unaltered from baseline). Therefore, although visual input is not required for the ability to perform the mental hand rotation task, it is able to compensate for sensory information lost during regional anesthesia. Sensory-visual phenomena often display the strength of vision in modulating perception of somatosensory input. Examples include mirror therapy for treatment of phantom limb pain, which involves visualization of a restored body image14; and the rubber-hand illusion, which involves perceptual displacement of touch from a subject's hidden hand to a visible dummy hand.15The convergence of multisensory afferent information with spatial memory may occur in regions such as the posterior parietal cortex, which is known to involve motor preparation and imagery, and spatial discriminatory aspects of pain processing.10,12,16The compensatory effects of visual input of a limb likely occur within these regions, where visual information counters the functional alterations produced by manipulated sensory input from the periphery.

In parallel with the effects of regional anesthesia in the study by Silva et al. ,2alterations in motor imagery and central body representation occur in chronic pain states, including patients with complex regional pain syndrome and chronic low-back pain.17,18Patients with complex regional pain syndrome display delayed responses when performing a mental hand imagery task that does not require actual movements of the affected limb.18These patients also demonstrate altered visuospatial perception, with their visual subjective body-midline representation shifted toward their affected side, and they perceive their affected limb as larger than it actually is.19,20Furthermore, patients with complex regional pain syndrome occasionally have pain that spreads from the initially affected region to the contralateral limb.21This occurs without any additional initiating neuropathic event and may be caused by bilateral spread of disinhibition within cortical representations of the body, similar to how a loss in inhibitory neurotransmitter function occurs in sensorimotor cortex after acute deafferentation.22Thus, altered processing in the central nervous system in chronic pain conditions appears to be closely related to and to affect mechanisms that subserve motor mental imagery and body representation.

In summary, there exists clear evidence that bilateral somatosensory and possibly some proprioceptive cues from the periphery contribute substantially to mental processes supporting motor imagery. Regional anesthesia alters both the perceptual and functional central representation of the body subsequent to its blockade of peripheral nerves. In the normal state, spatiotemporal maps in the cortex, which are highly dependent on incoming sensory input, are being actively referenced and compared with the current afferent sensory information to create the overall perception of a limb. During regional anesthesia and in disease states, such as chronic pain, the mismatch of what information the central nervous system expects to receive versus  what it actually receives produces physiologic and functional alterations at multiple levels of the central nervous system. This mismatch may also produce sensory illusions. Vision functions as an alternative source of peripheral input that can partially rectify the mismatch between peripheral input and its central counterparts by acting on higher-order regions of cortex involved in the imagery component of the motor imagery task.

By involving patients scheduled for preoperative regional anesthesia, the study by Silva et al.  2is an elegant example of the valuable opportunity for conducting basic science investigations in conjunction with clinical procedures. Future studies should take advantage of this approach to elucidate further the complex relationship between peripheral afferent information and central representation of the body and how it is disrupted in disease states. Furthermore, states of increased brain plasticity occur after regional anesthesia and may be useful for priming the central nervous system so that it responds more readily and quickly to therapies aimed at reforming neuronal connections.23,24Through continued investigation of its extensive effects within the nervous system, regional anesthesia may evolve to be much more than a routine, yet vital, clinical procedure.

Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, North Carolina. kmartucc@wfubmc.edu

1.
Arner S, Lindblom U, Meyerson BA, Molander C: Prolonged relief of neuralgia after regional anesthetic blocks: A call for further experimental and systematic clinical studies. Pain 1990; 43:287–97
2.
Silva S, Loubinoux I, Olivier M, Bataille B, Fourcade O, Samii K, Jeannerod M, Démonet J-F: Impaired visual hand recognition in preoperative patients during brachial plexus anesthesia: Importance of peripheral neural input for mental representation of the hand. Anesthesiology 2011; 114:126–34
3.
Gandevia SC, Phegan CM: Perceptual distortions of the human body image produced by local anaesthesia, pain, and cutaneous stimulation. J Physiol 1999; 514:609–16
4.
Waberski TD, Gobbel é R, Kawohl W, Cordes C, Buchner H: Immediate cortical reorganization after local anesthetic block of the thumb: Source localization of somatosensory evoked potentials in human subjects. Neurosci Lett 2003; 347:151–4
5.
Merzenich MM, Nelson RJ, Stryker MP, Cynader MS, Schoppmann A, Zook JM: Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys. J Comp Neurol 1984; 224:591–605
6.
Mogilner A, Grossman JA, Ribary U, Joliot M, Volkmann J, Rapaport D, Beasley RW, Llinás RR: Somatosensory cortical plasticity in adult humans revealed by magnetoencephalography. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1993; 90:3593–7
7.
Pons TP, Garraghty PE, Ommaya AK, Kaas JH, Taub E, Mishkin M: Massive cortical reorganization after sensory deafferentation in adult macaques. Science 1991; 252: 1857–60
8.
Jeannerod M, Decety J: Mental motor imagery: A window into the representational stages of action. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1995; 5:727–32
9.
Werhahn KJ, Mortensen J, Kaelin-Lang A, Boroojerdi B, Cohen LG: Cortical excitability changes induced by deafferentation of the contralateral hemisphere. Brain 2002; 125:1402–13
10.
Parsons LM, Fox PT, Downs JH, Glass T, Hirsch TB, Martin CC, Jerabek PA, Lancaster JL: Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET. Nature 1995; 375:54–8
11.
Stephan KM, Fink GR, Passingham RE, Silbersweig D, Ceballos-Baumann AO, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS: Functional anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. J Neurophysiol 1995; 73:373–86
12.
Solodkin A, Hlustik P, Chen EE, Small SL: Fine modulation in network activation during motor execution and motor imagery. Cereb Cortex 2004; 14:1246–55
13.
Jeannerod M: Mental imagery in the motor context. Neuropsychologia 1995; 33:1419–32
14.
Ramachandran VS, Hirstein W: The perception of phantom limbs. The D. O. Hebb lecture. Brain 1998; 121:1603–30
15.
Botvinick M, Cohen J: Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature 1998; 391:756
16.
Oshiro Y, Quevedo AS, McHaffie JG, Kraft RA, Coghill RC: Brain mechanisms supporting spatial discrimination of pain. J Neurosci 2007; 27:3388–94
17.
Parsons LM, Gabrieli JD, Phelps EA, Gazzaniga MS: Cerebrally lateralized mental representations of hand shape and movement. J Neurosci 1998; 18:6539–48
18.
Schwoebel J, Friedman R, Duda N, Coslett HB: Pain and the body schema: Evidence for peripheral effects on mental representations of movement. Brain 2001; 124:2098–104
19.
Moseley GL: Distorted body image in complex regional pain syndrome. Neurology 2005; 65:773
20.
Uematsu H, Sumitani M, Yozu A, Otake Y, Shibata M, Mashimo T, Miyauchi S: Complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) impairs visuospatial perception, whereas post-herpetic neuralgia does not: Possible implications for supraspinal mechanism of CRPS. Ann Acad Med Singapore 2009; 38:931–6
21.
Veldman PH, Goris RJ: Multiple reflex sympathetic dystrophy. Which patients are at risk for developing a recurrence of reflex sympathetic dystrophy in the same or another limb. Pain 1996; 64:463–6
22.
Levy LM, Ziemann U, Chen R, Cohen LG: Rapid modulation of GABA in sensorimotor cortex induced by acute deafferentation. Ann Neurol 2002; 52:755–61
23.
Björkman A, Rosén B, van Westen D, Larsson EM, Lundborg G: Acute improvement of contralateral hand function after deafferentation. Neuroreport 2004; 15:1861–5
24.
Hassan-Zadeh R, Lajevardi L, Esfahani AR, Kamali M: Improvement of hand sensibility after selective temporary anaesthesia in combination with sensory re-education. NeuroRehabilitation 2009; 24:383–6