To the Editor:

We read with great interest the results of the largest Bispectral Index (BIS) monitoring study ever performed, which was published in the October 2012 issue of Anesthesiology.1  No significant difference in intraoperative awareness with explicit recall was detected between BIS and anesthetic concentration protocols (0.08 vs. 0.12%, P = 0.48) in an unselected surgical population of 21,601 patients. Initial multicenter studies suggested that BIS monitoring could reduce the incidence of explicit recall in high-risk surgical patients,2  but later studies that compared BIS monitoring with carefully guided dosing schemes with audible alerts for low concentrations of the anesthetic failed to demonstrate such benefit.3,4  Now, this negative result was corroborated in a “normal” population (BIS < 60 vs. minimum alveolar concentration > 0.5). What went wrong? Why does BIS monitoring not perform better?

We believe that there are two main reasons. First, the suggested intraoperative “therapeutic window” (BIS 40–60) to guide anesthetic dosing is not optimal for preventing unintended awareness and is most probably dictated by manufacturer’s aspiration to not to prolong awakening after anesthesia. The scientific evidence that BIS should be kept below 60 to prevent awareness is extremely weak if not totally nonexistent. We find it incomprehensible that this fundamental issue is not dealt with in the literature. Every anesthesiologist who has used BIS monitoring knows that BIS level 60 represents a labile “depth of anesthesia,” and even a small surgical or other irritation can lead to arousal and awakening. Deepening anesthesia induces characteristic electroencephalographic changes, and lowering the reference range would undoubtedly improve the sensitivity of BIS to prevent awareness despite the wide interpatient variability in its concentration–response curves and partially distinct electroencephalographic effects of different anesthetic agents. Because of the nonlinear behavior of BIS,5  keeping it close to 40 is actually relatively easy.

Our recent positron emission tomography imaging study with anesthetized healthy subjects suggests another reason for the poor performance of BIS. The emergence of consciousness after anesthetic-induced unconsciousness, as assessed with a motor response to a spoken command, was found to be associated with activation of deep, primitive brain structures rather than the evolutionary younger neocortex.6  Unexpectedly, activation of these central core structures was enough for the arousal and behavioral expression of subjective awareness. Because BIS is based on cortical electroencephalographic measurement (i.e., measuring electrical signals on the surface of the scalp that arise from the brain’s cortical surface), these results help to understand why BIS fails in differentiating the conscious and unconscious states in the subtle transition phase during emergence7  and why patient awareness during general anesthesia may not always be detected.

We also have two minor comments concerning the article by Mashour et al.1  and the accompanied editorial.8  On the basis of the detailed description on page 719, the most important principle of randomization and the randomized controlled trial paradigm may have been breached; in a randomized trial, the investigator should not know the treatment/intervention allocation before patient recruitment. Or have we misunderstood the procedure? In addition, the editorial included a funny flaw: BIS spectroscopy. Surely, we are not able to scope anything with BIS.

1.
Mashour
GA
,
Shanks
A
,
Tremper
KK
,
Kheterpal
S
,
Turner
CR
,
Ramachandran
SK
,
Picton
P
,
Schueller
C
,
Morris
M
,
Vandervest
JC
,
Lin
N
,
Avidan
MS
:
Prevention of intraoperative awareness with explicit recall in an unselected surgical population: A randomized comparative effectiveness trial.
Anesthesiology
2012
;
117
:
717
25
2.
Myles
PS
,
Leslie
K
,
McNeil
J
,
Forbes
A
,
Chan
MT
:
Bispectral index monitoring to prevent awareness during anaesthesia: The B-Aware randomised controlled trial.
Lancet
2004
;
363
:
1757
63
3.
Avidan
MS
,
Zhang
L
,
Burnside
BA
,
Finkel
KJ
,
Searleman
AC
,
Selvidge
JA
,
Saager
L
,
Turner
MS
,
Rao
S
,
Bottros
M
,
Hantler
C
,
Jacobsohn
E
,
Evers
AS
:
Anesthesia awareness and the bispectral index.
N Engl J Med
2008
;
358
:
1097
108
4.
Avidan
MS
,
Jacobsohn
E
,
Glick
D
,
Burnside
BA
,
Zhang
L
,
Villafranca
A
,
Karl
L
,
Kamal
S
,
Torres
B
,
O’Connor
M
,
Evers
AS
,
Gradwohl
S
,
Lin
N
,
Palanca
BJ
,
Mashour
GA
;
BAG-RECALL Research Group
:
Prevention of intraoperative awareness in a high-risk surgical population.
N Engl J Med
2011
;
365
:
591
600
5.
Olofsen
E
,
Dahan
A
:
The dynamic relationship between end-tidal sevoflurane and isoflurane concentrations and bispectral index and spectral edge frequency of the electroencephalogram.
Anesthesiology
1999
;
90
:
1345
53
6.
Långsjö
JW
,
Alkire
MT
,
Kaskinoro
K
,
Hayama
H
,
Maksimow
A
,
Kaisti
KK
,
Aalto
S
,
Aantaa
R
,
Jääskeläinen
SK
,
Revonsuo
A
,
Scheinin
H
:
Returning from oblivion: Imaging the neural core of consciousness.
J Neurosci
2012
;
32
:
4935
43
7.
Kaskinoro
K
,
Maksimow
A
,
Långsjö
J
,
Aantaa
R
,
Jääskeläinen
S
,
Kaisti
K
,
Särkelä
M
,
Scheinin
H
:
Wide inter-individual variability of bispectral index and spectral entropy at loss of consciousness during increasing concentrations of dexmedetomidine, propofol, and sevoflurane.
Br J Anaesth
2011
;
107
:
573
80
8.
Memtsoudis
SG
,
Liu
SS
:
Bispectral index versus minimum alveolar concentration for prevention of intraoperative awareness: Does a practical controlled trial provide CERtainty?
Anesthesiology
2012
;
117
:
693
5