Background:

The Postoperative Quality of Recovery Scale found lower than anticipated recovery in the cognitive domain. The definition of cognitive recovery did not allow for performance variability, and may have been too sensitive. This study aimed to examine variability in cognitive performance in volunteers.

Methods:

One hundred forty-three volunteers completed the cognitive domain questions at baseline, after 15 min and 40 min, and on days 1 and 3. Delivery via face-to-face interview was conducted for the first three measurements, and then randomized for day 1 and 3 measurements (face-to-face only, telephone only, telephone then face-to-face, face-to-face then telephone).

Results:

All volunteers answered orientation correctly. Mean change scores for other tests were positive, indicating a modest learning effect. There were no significant differences between methods of delivery (all P > 0.05). Due to variability in volunteers’ performances, the authors propose a new scoring system to introduce a tolerance factor in scoring cognitive recovery. The proposed revised change from baseline scores are: orientation 0 or higher, digits forward −2 or higher, digits back −1 or higher, word recall −3 or higher, and word generation −3 or higher. This resulted in approximately 95% volunteers classed as “recovered” for each test item, and recovery for the domains ranged from 82.6 to 89.1%. The initial feasibility study was reanalyzed and cognitive recovery increased at all assessment times. At 3 days, cognitive recovery was found to increase from 33.5 to 86.4%.

Conclusion:

The authors recommend adoption of the new method for scoring cognitive recovery in the Postoperative Quality of Recovery Scale. Telephone or face-to-face delivery was equivalent and either method can be reliably applied.

What We Already Know about This Topic
  • The Postoperative Quality of Recovery Scale, published in Anesthesiology in 2010, found lower than anticipated recovery in the cognitive domain.

  • One hundred forty-three volunteers completed the Postoperative Quality of Recovery Scale cognitive domain questions at baseline, 15 min, 40 min, and 1 and 3 days. Delivery was face-to-face for the first three measurements, and then randomized for day 1 and 3 measurements to combinations of face-to-face and telephone interviews.

What This Article Tells Us That Is New
  • The investigators propose a new scoring system that includes performance tolerance such that more than 80% of subjects are considered recovered in the cognitive domain at 3 days.

  • There were no important differences between methods of delivery; telephone administration of Postoperative Quality of Recovery Scale is, thus, valid.

THE Postoperative Quality of Recovery Scale (PQRS) is a tool to measure quality of recovery after surgery and anesthesia.1  Recovery is measured in five domains (physiological, nociceptive—pain and nausea, emotive—anxiety and depression, activities of daily living, and cognition). Recovery is defined as a return to baseline scores (presurgery) or better. For most of the domains, the answer choices are either a 3- or 5-point Likert scale. However, for the cognitive domain, there is a wider range of possible performance for some of the tests. To assess cognition, five questions are used (orientation to name, place, and date of birth, digits forward, digits back, word generation, and word recall), which are derived from formal neurocognitive tests used to assess cognitive performance.2  Parallel forms, containing different number and word choices for the questions, are frequently used to minimize the learning effect, which is prevalent in neurocognitive testing, but do not always remove these completely.2 

In the PQRS publication,1  we reported data from 701 patients undergoing our feasibility study. The proportion of cognitive recovery, defined as return to baseline, was low, with only 33.5% recovery by day 3. As part of the ongoing validation of the PQRS, we conducted a volunteer study to identify the performance variability of the cognitive tests. Normal volunteers were not expected to demonstrate neurocognitive decline during a 3-day period, if anything, they would be expected to show some level of improved performance through learning. However, they may also have deterioration in performance due to extraneous factors, such as fatigue. It was considered possible that the absolute definition of recovery used in the PQRS, may be overly sensitive for measurement of cognition, as it did not allow for any performance variability.

The aim of the study was to measure performance variability and test reliability over 3 days, using the PQRS in the cognitive domain as well as to assess the method of delivery on cognitive performance in healthy volunteers.

The study was performed in two centers, at the University of Melbourne and University College, London. Human research ethics committee approval was obtained from the Human Research Ethics Committees of The University of Melbourne (Melbourne, Australia), and The University College (London, United Kingdom). After informed written consent, 143 volunteers without cognitive disability were recruited. The volunteers were asked whether they had any previous medical or learning disorder that would indicate cognitive disability.

PQRS cognition testing was conducted on five occasions. After recruitment, baseline testing was performed, followed by a repeat at 15 and 40 min. These three measurement points were conducted via face-to-face interview. Testing was repeated on days 1 and 3. Volunteers were randomized into four groups, using a computer generated random sequence, according to telephone or face-to-face interview for days 1 and 3 time periods. This protocol attempted to replicate the timings used in the PQRS feasibility study.1  The sequence of testing for the four groups on days 1 and 3 were: telephone then telephone, telephone then face-to-face, face-to-face then face-to-face, and face-to-face then telephone.

The five cognitive tests used in the PQRS are described in table 1. The same parallel forms and time points, which were used in the PQRS feasibility study, are used in this study. The score for each question at each measurement time period were subtracted from the baseline scores to produce a change score.

Table 1.

Description of the Five Cognitive Tests Used in the Postoperative Quality of Recovery Scale

Description of the Five Cognitive Tests Used in the Postoperative Quality of Recovery Scale
Description of the Five Cognitive Tests Used in the Postoperative Quality of Recovery Scale

In the PQRS feasibility study,1  a convenience sample of patients (n = 701) were recruited into the study if they were 6 yr or older, undergoing elective surgery under general anesthesia, and able to complete the testing. Exclusion criteria were: (1) current psychiatric disturbance, or (2) undergoing neurosurgery, which could impair the patients’ ability to participate in the assessment. A wide range of surgical cases were included and selection was by convenience sampling. A reanalysis of the cognitive domain recovery was performed, using the results of the human volunteer study, which specifically involved adjustment by the use of a tolerance factor to each cognitive area. Cognitive domain recovery still required recovery in all five areas.

Statistical Methods

Cognitive score values are expressed as mean ± SD. Comparison between delivery methods was performed using repeated measures ANOVA for between-group (group × time) interactions (SPSS V19; IBM, Chicago, IL). P value less than 0.05 was considered significant. Reliability was assessed using Cronbach α, comparing the change score for each cognitive test over four recovery time periods.

The proportion of volunteers recovered for each test was calculated for change scores of 0 or higher, −1 or higher, −2 or higher, and −3 or higher, with the intention of achieving approximately 2 SDs of the population of volunteers to be classed as “recovered” for each test. The sample size was based on a repeated measured ANOVA design for four groups, to account for different delivery methods of face-to-face and telephone delivery. With an estimated SD of 1.0 between measures, α = 0.05, power of 80%, and a moderate effect size of 0.5, the minimal sample size was 32 per group. Due to the difference in score values between the five tests, a more conservative estimate was used and a target of 45 volunteers per group was planned.

One hundred forty-three volunteers participated in the study. Due to logistical difficulties in recruitment at one site, only 143 of the projected 180 volunteers were recruited. The group consisted of telephone-telephone (36), face-to-face then face-to-face (40), telephone then face-to-face (30), and face-to-face then telephone (37). The age was 37 ± 17 (range 17–92 yr) and years of education was 16.4 ± 3.2 yr (range 5–30). Of the volunteers 61 were men, and 82 were women.

Baseline values and changes scores are shown in table 2. All volunteers scored 3 on the orientation subtest (maximum score), but performance was variable for the other tests. The change scores for each cognitive test and for each delivery method are shown in figure 1. There were no significant differences between groups defined by delivery for any cognitive tests. The mean change scores showed a small positive value, indicating a learning effect evident at the first repeated assessment, but this did not continue to increase with subsequent testing. The groups were combined for subsequent analysis.

Fig. 1.

The mean change scores are shown for each group of volunteers over four time periods, for digits forward, digits back, word recall, and word generation. Error bars indicate 1 SD. P is the repeated measured ANOVA for group × time comparisons. T15 is conducted 15 min, T40 is conducted 40 min, D1 and D3 are conducted on days 1 and 3 after baseline.

Fig. 1.

The mean change scores are shown for each group of volunteers over four time periods, for digits forward, digits back, word recall, and word generation. Error bars indicate 1 SD. P is the repeated measured ANOVA for group × time comparisons. T15 is conducted 15 min, T40 is conducted 40 min, D1 and D3 are conducted on days 1 and 3 after baseline.

Close modal
Table 2.

Baseline Scores and Change Scores

Baseline Scores and Change Scores
Baseline Scores and Change Scores

The incidence of recovery in normal volunteers for tests other than orientation ranged from 67.1 to 86.1% and is shown in table 3. To achieve approximately 2 SDs of the cohort classed as “recovered” for each of the tests, the change scores to define recovery were altered to: orientation 0 or higher (unchanged), digits forwards −2 or higher, digits back −1 or higher, word recall −3 or higher, and word generation −3 or higher. The original and new recovery proportions for each test and time period are shown in table 3.

Table 3.

Proportion of Volunteers (%) Scored as Recovered Using the Original and New Scoring Methods

Proportion of Volunteers (%) Scored as Recovered Using the Original and New Scoring Methods
Proportion of Volunteers (%) Scored as Recovered Using the Original and New Scoring Methods

With the introduction of the tolerance factor, patients with baseline scores that are equal to or below the tolerance factor would automatically score as “recovered.” The proportion of volunteers with baseline scores of 3 or lesser for digits back was 1.4%, 2 or lesser for digits back was 2.8%, 4 or lesser for word recall was 2.1%, and 4 or lesser for word generation was 0%. As digits back is a more difficult test, the proportion of volunteers with baseline scores of 3 or lesser was 32%, and the decision was made to reduce the tolerance factor to baseline −1 for the digits back test as a compromise between accuracy and feasibility (to allow most of the patients to complete the test). The recovery rates using the original definition of “return to baseline values or better” and the revised scoring for the whole cognitive domain is shown in figure 2.

Fig. 2.

The proportion of volunteers who were scored as recovered in the cognitive domain is shown for the original and new scoring methods. The green shaded box indicates a range where recovery should be considered as very good.

Fig. 2.

The proportion of volunteers who were scored as recovered in the cognitive domain is shown for the original and new scoring methods. The green shaded box indicates a range where recovery should be considered as very good.

Close modal

A subanalysis of baseline scores and change scores was conducted for patients less than 50 yr versus patients 50 yr or more. The only difference in baseline scores between groups was for the word recall where older patients scored lower than younger patients (mean [SD], 5.9 [1.8] vs. 7.8 [1.8]; P < 0.001). Repeated measures analysis of change scores between older and younger groups was not different for any of the cognitive tests (all P > 0.05).

The reliability of individual cognitive tests was acceptable with Cronbach α values of 0.837 for digits forward, 0.801 for digits back, 0.841 for word recall, and 0.815 for word generation.

The cognitive recovery from the 701 patient feasibility study1  was reanalyzed using the new scoring system, and shown in figure 3. Patients with low baseline scores (digits forward <3, digits back <2, word recall <4, and word generation <4) were excluded from analysis, leaving 533 patients for analysis. Overall cognitive recovery rates in patients attempting the PQRS increased from 2.7, 8, 28.7, and 33.5% at 15 min, 40 min, day 1 and day 3 after surgery, to 22.7, 45.4, 83.5, and 86.4%, respectively.

Fig. 3.

The proportion of patients recovered in the cognitive domain (all tests recovered) is shown for the feasibility study of the Postoperative Quality of Recovery Scale, 1 using both the original and new scoring methods.

Fig. 3.

The proportion of patients recovered in the cognitive domain (all tests recovered) is shown for the feasibility study of the Postoperative Quality of Recovery Scale, 1 using both the original and new scoring methods.

Close modal

This study showed that there is variability in performance in all the cognitive tests except for orientation (which has a ceiling effect) in normal volunteers not undergoing surgery. Apart from orientation, the other cognitive tests showed sufficient variability that the strict definition of recovery “return to baseline values or better” for these cognitive questions resulted in failure of recovery in approximately 25% of volunteers for individual cognitive tests, and more than 50% failed to recover in the cognitive domain. Our premise is that volunteers should not have substantially lower cognitive performance over the 3 days of testing. By introducing a tolerance value into the scoring system, the average recovery rates approached 2 SDs of the population for each test, and recovery exceeded 80% for the cognitive domain. It is our recommendation that this new scoring system be adopted to define recovery for the cognitive domain of the PQRS.

The method of delivery of the PQRS, whether by telephone or face-to-face did not significantly affect cognitive performance. Use of telephone interview after discharge improves the feasibility of conducting follow-up PQRS measurements, as patients do not need to return to the hospital for face-to-face interviews.

Other domains of the PQRS use a 3- or 5-point Likert scale to rate items, such as pain, nausea, anxiety, depression, or activities of daily living. Although subject to some variability, these subjective reports by patients are likely to accurately rate improvement or worsening of pain or nausea, for example, from the previous exposure. Cognitive testing, however, can have variability of performance within normal volunteers, as was shown in this study, leading to potential inaccuracy of the test. Although older volunteers had lower baseline scores for the word generation test, there was no significant difference in change scores over time between younger and older patients. Factors such as the time of day, fatigue levels, situational exposure, and other distractions could affect cognitive performance.2  If the strict definition of return to baseline values or better is applied to the cognitive domain, then the tool may be considered as too sensitive and will yield many false-positives, with the result that many patients who may have recovered would be classed as not recovered. Thus, the low incidence of cognitive recovery in the PQRS feasibility study1  may have been exaggerated due to the strict definition applied. The introduction of a tolerance factor, as described in this article, enables the tool to account for natural variability of performance and is likely to reflect more accurately the true incidence of recovery from surgery and anesthesia.

Recovery for the whole domain will be lower than recovery for individual tests, as failure in any one of the five tests results in failure of recovery for the domain. Even though individual test recovery rates exceed 90%, and mostly exceeded 95%, the recovery for the whole domain ranged between 82.6 and 89.1%. Therefore, a recovery rate exceeding 80% is considered more realistic of good recovery than a recovery rate of 95–100%. In figures 2 and 3, we have added a colored box from 80 to 100% in order to illustrate that recovery in this range should be considered good recovery.

The size of the tolerance factor is a balance between accuracy and feasibility. If it is too small, then the tool could have an excessive false-positive (failure to recover) rate. However, if too high, then it will lose discrimination ability and potentially have an excessive false-negative rate. The accuracy as determined by the Cronbach α was acceptable for each test. Another factor in determining the size of the tolerance factor is that the baseline scores have to exceed the tolerance factor, otherwise patients would automatically be scored as recovered. These patients would need to be excluded from recovery analysis in the cognitive domain. We believe that it was acceptable for less than 5% of volunteers to be excluded for each cognitive test because of low baseline scores. In the case of digits back, the proportion of volunteers excluded rose from 2.8% for a tolerance factor of baseline −1, to 32% for a tolerance of baseline −2. The larger tolerance factor would render the test unfeasible due to exclusion of so many patients, and the decision was made to use the lower tolerance factor. We recommend excluding patients whose baseline values are less than the tolerance factor.

The tolerance factors are similar to the SD of baseline values. This is a similar concept to that used in assessing postoperative cognitive dysfunction, where significant change in a cognitive test is typically more than 1 SD from baseline values.3–5  When utilizing these new measures, the incidence of cognitive recovery was recalculated in the original PQRS feasibility study increased proportionally over time with 86.4% recovery at 3 days after surgery, and is more consistent with clinical expectation. This places recovery from day 1 in the “range of good recovery.”

Variability of performance in cognitive testing is not an inherent quality of the PQRS, but rather an inherent quality of all neurocognitive testing. The PQRS tests are based on conventional neurocognitive tests, all of which are subject to variability. In addition to the variability of patient performance, there is the added variability that patients can recover and then lapse into a worse state. Furthermore, the questions test different aspects of cognition yet these are collapsed to produce a dichotomized outcome of recovery or postoperative cognitive dysfunction. The very nature of cognition testing is, therefore, subject to inaccuracy. There are many strategies used to minimize inaccuracy, as we have described for the PQRS, or the use of mathematical correction factors to adjust for baseline variability. However, they are all techniques to reduce inaccuracy and cannot eliminate the inherent inaccuracies and variability associated with neurocognitive testing.

So, how should the reader interpret cognitive recovery using the PQRS? First, we recommend the concept of “a range of good recovery,” which is a group recovery above 80%. We also advise the reader to be cautious in interpreting data where small differences are observed. A small difference (e.g., 72 vs. 77%) could be “statistically significant” but potentially fall into the overlap of inaccuracies for each group. This is the same principle that the reader would apply to measurements that have variability of performance or subjective assessment (such as pain, delirium, or satisfaction scales). We also recommend that a single time point of recovery is less informative than the profile of recovery over multiple time periods. Although single time points are often used to determine sample size, it is easier to discriminate differences when measured over multiple time points, as well as detecting the time period when recovery plateaus or becomes equivalent between groups. Good trial design will improve the ability to discriminate between groups, such as adequate sample size, randomization, and the minimization of confounders. Discriminant validation studies are in progress with the PQRS and will add to the confidence in use of the scale.

Our study has several limitations. Our recruitment was less than intended due to logistical difficulties, although in all but one group, the group size exceeded our minimal sample size estimate. It is possible that a small difference could exist between the groups that was not detected, resulting in a small risk of type II error, especially as the study was powered to detect a moderate difference between groups. As our aim was to assess variability in normal volunteers, it is possible that the degree of accuracy may be different in patients with cognitive disability, or in the postoperative setting. Similarly, it is possible that the use of telephone versus face-to-face survey methods could vary in specific patient populations or at different times in the postoperative period.

We recommend adoption of the new scoring system for the cognitive domain of the PQRS, exclusion of patients with baseline cognitive scores below the tolerance factor, and recognition of recovery for the cognitive domain exceeding 80% as good recovery. Telephone or face-to-face delivery was equivalent.

The authors thank the many volunteers who participated in the study. We thank Jan Stygall, MSc, Senior Research Fellow (now retired), Unit of Behavioural Medicine, University College London, London, United Kingdom, for contribution to patient recruitment and data collation.

1.
Royse
CF
,
Newman
S
,
Chung
F
,
Stygall
J
,
McKay
RE
,
Boldt
J
,
Servin
FS
,
Hurtado
I
,
Hannallah
R
,
Yu
B
,
Wilkinson
DJ
:
Development and feasibility of a scale to assess postoperative recovery: The post-operative quality recovery scale.
Anesthesiology
2010
;
113
:
892
05
2.
Lezak
M
,
Loring
D
,
Hannay
H
,
Joscher
J
:
Neuropsychological Assessment
, 4th edition.
New York
,
Oxford University Press
,
2004
3.
Murkin
JM
,
Newman
SP
,
Stump
DA
,
Blumenthal
JA
:
Statement of consensus on assessment of neurobehavioral outcomes after cardiac surgery.
Ann Thorac Surg
1995
;
59
:
1289
95
4.
Funder
KS
,
Steinmetz
J
,
Rasmussen
LS
:
Methodological issues of postoperative cognitive dysfunction research.
Semin Cardiothorac Vasc Anesth
2010
;
14
:
119
22
5.
Rudolph
JL
,
Schreiber
KA
,
Culley
DJ
,
McGlinchey
RE
,
Crosby
G
,
Levitsky
S
,
Marcantonio
ER
:
Measurement of post-operative cognitive dysfunction after cardiac surgery: A systematic review.
Acta Anaesthesiol Scand
2010
;
54
:
663
77