Background

Several recent studies have analyzed the relationship between pharmacokinetic parameters and the rate of decrease in concentration after discontinuation of a continuous drug infusion. Although these studies have clarified our understanding of those aspects of pharmacokinetics most relevant to anesthesia practice, they do not directly address the issue of the duration of drug effect, which will be a function of both pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic variables. This paper extends these concepts by presenting a method to unify pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics in a measure of duration of drug effect that is applicable when the drug effect is assessed in a binary, response/no response fashion.

Methods: The parameter proposed to quantify duration of drug effect is the area under the curve expressing probability of drug effect as a function of time after the agent is discontinued. This parameter is denoted the mean effect time. It is calculated using the logistic (or Hill) equation to relate the probability of drug effect to drug concentration, which in turn can be calculated as a function of time by pharmacokinetic simulation. Mean effect times were calculated for sufentanil, alfentanil, propofol, and midazolam using the logistic equation describing recovery and by assuming that drug blood concentrations during maintenance of anesthesia were sufficient to reduce the probability of responsiveness to surgical stimulation to 10%(C90). Published pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic parameters were used for these calculations. These results were compared to the relevant decrement times (as defined in this paper, the time required for the concentration to decrease from C90to the concentration at which 50% of patients are responsive and/or able to maintain adequate ventilation, denoted C50). It was assumed that C90and C50were independent variables.

Results: Mean effect times for midazolam and propofol, for which the steepness parameter delta for recovery (responsiveness and adequate ventilation) is less than 4, are significantly greater than the decrement time. Mean effect times for sufentanil and alfentanil (delta = 6 and 10, respectively) are close to decrement times. The discrepancy between mean effect time and decrement time becomes greater as the duration of drug administration increases. The incorporation of pharmacokinetic variability into the calculations had little effect on the results.

Conclusions: Context-sensitive half-times or other decrement times have been shown to be the most useful measures of the kinetics of drug concentrations. Mean effect time may be a useful concept for understanding the recovery from drug effects.

THE duration of drug effect is a function of both pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic properties. The influence of pharmacokinetic variables has been significantly clarified by papers by Shafer and Varvel and Hughes et al. These authors have demonstrated, using computer simulation, that the rate of decrease of either the effect site (Shafer and Varvel) or plasma (Hughes et al.) concentration after continuous intravenous infusion cannot be simply related to any one pharmacokinetic parameter and is highly dependent on the duration of infusion. Hughes et al. introduced the term context-sensitive half-time to indicate the time necessary for a 50% decrease in plasma drug concentration. This parameter is a function of the "context", i.e., the duration of drug administration before its discontinuation. This concept can be generalized to "decrement times," whereby, for example, the 80% decrement time is the time needed for the plasma or effect site concentration to decrease by 80%. The duration of drug effect can be identified with the appropriate decrement time if the drug effect can be measured by a continuous variable for which there is a well defined value of recovery. An example is recovery from muscle relaxants, as was demonstrated by Kern. However, in anesthesiology, we often deal with binary data, i.e., the patient is responsive or not, ventilation is adequate or not, and so on. At any specific drug concentration, there is a probability of drug effect, and it usually is assumed that this probability can be described within the framework of the logistic distribution as P = Cdelta/(Cdelta+ C50delta), where P is the probability of drug effect, C is the drug concentration, C50is the concentration at which the probability of drug effect is 50%, and delta is a parameter determining the steepness of the relationship. If delta is very large, the probability of drug effect is nearly 1 when C is larger than C50(even if only slightly larger) and nearly 0 when C is less than C50. In this case, the duration of drug effect will be the decrement time between the drug concentration maintained during anesthesia and C50, because the transition between effect and no effect is very sharp. However, for smaller values of delta, this may not be true, because there will still be a finite probability of drug effect even when the concentration is less than C50. In this paper, I extend the context-sensitive half-time/decrement time concept and propose a parameter, mean effect time (MET), which can be used to quantify the duration of drug effect when dealing with binary (response or no response) data and consider the implications of this approach for several commonly used intravenous anesthetic agents.

Mean effect time (MET) is defined as

formula

where P(t) is the probability of drug effect at time t. The mathematical rationale for this parameter is as follows. The probability of drug effect, P, is a function of drug concentration, C, and because C is time-dependent, P may be viewed as a function of time. During the time span from t to t + dt, the probability of drug effect is P(t). The sum of drug effect for all time spans is then given by integrating P(t) over t. Note, because P(t)(as a probability) is unitless, the unit of MET is time.

An intuitive understanding of this parameter is illustrated in Figure 1for the hypothetical recovery from a continuous infusion of fentanyl that has maintained a plasma concentration of 10 ng/ml for 60 min. F1-24presents the plasma concentration of fentanyl as a function of time after the infusion is discontinued, as well as the probability of drug effect predicted for these plasma concentrations using the logistic equation with C50= 5 and delta = 2 or 10. It is seen that the area under the probability of effect curve when delta = 10 is nearly rectangular. Because the probability of effect is nearly 1 when C is greater than C50and nearly 0 when C is less than C50, the area under the curve, which is MET by definition, is 25 min, a value nearly equal to the time needed for the plasma concentration to decrease from 10 ng/ml (the concentration during anesthesia maintenance) to 5 ng/ml (C50), i.e., the context-sensitive half-time (23 min). In contrast to this case for fentanyl, where delta is relatively large, note the probability of drug effect as a function of time (F1-24) when delta = 2. In this case, the curve is not nearly as rectangular. The area under the curve (MET) is 130 min, whereas the decrement time from 10 to 5 ng/ml is only 23 min. In this case, MET is considerably larger than the context-sensitive half-time because there is still a significant probability of drug effect when the concentration is less than C50.

In the above example, MET was calculated assuming the fentanyl concentration during maintenance of anesthesia was 10 ng/ml. This was an arbitrary assumption. Clearly, the value of MET will depend on the drug concentration during maintenance. For the simulations in this paper, drug concentrations during maintenance were calculated by assuming that the probability of response to surgical stimulation was only 10%(this concentration will be denoted C90). This is comparable to the clinical practice of maintaining patients with 1.3 MAC of an inhaled agent, rather than 1.0 MAC, to avoid the possibility of movement in 50% of patients. C90was calculated by using the drug concentration-response relationship for maintenance of surgical anesthesia, where available (this, in general, may be different from the drug concentration-response curve describing recovery of responsiveness and adequate spontaneous ventilation). Pharmacodynamic parameters were taken from Wessen et al. for propofol (using their data for propofol supplemented with alfentanil), from Jacobs et al. for midazolam, from Sear and Gavaghan for sufentanil,* and from Ausems et al. for alfentanil (calculating C90from their data for upper-abdominal surgery). Calculations were performed with an IBM-compatible personal computer using an Excel 5.0 spreadsheet. Plasma concentrations after the discontinuation of bolus-elimination transfer-type infusions were simulated by convoluting the infusion rate with the unit disposition function, as described by Hughes et al. The term bolus-elimination transfer infusion refers to a dosing regimen comprised of a bolus followed immediately by an infusion(s) with exponentially declining rate(s) and is designed to achieve and maintain a constant plasma concentration. The unit disposition functions, which describe the concentration as a function of time after a unit bolus dose, were assumed to have the form udf (t)= A1exp(-k1sup * t)+ A2exp(-k2sup * t)+ A3exp(-k3sup * t), and the pharmacokinetic parameters used in these functions were taken from Hughes et al. for fentanyl, sufentanil, alfentanil, midazolam, and propofol. The logistic equation P = Cdelta/(Cdelta+ C sub 50delta) was used to calculate the probability of drug effect (P) at any concentration C, and MET was calculated by integrating P over time using the Euler technique with a step size of 15 s for 1,000 min after discontinuing the bolus-elimination transfer-type infusion and directly integrating the terminal term tail of the curve by assuming the concentration was well approximated by the terminal exponential term and that P could be approximated as (C/C50)deltawhen C was much less than C50.

The simulations performed as described above assumed "average" pharmacokinetic parameters and do not take into account pharmacokinetic variability. It is shown in the appendix that pharmacokinetic variability can be approximately accounted for by assuming that the probability of drug effect is better described by an extension of the logistic equation;

formula

where < C > is the average concentration (as a function of time); K is a complicated function of < C >, C50, and delta (derived in the appendix); and varcis the variance of C in the population at any given time. Simulations were performed by assuming that varccould be described by a simple constant coefficient of variation model, where varc= alpha2< C >2and alpha2was assumed to be 0.2.

Simulations also were performed in which MET was calculated by assuming that the probability of drug effect is related to the effect-site concentration, using the logistic equation (but not taking into account pharmacokinetic variability). The effect-site concentration was calculated as described by Shafer and Varvel, with values of keo(the blood effect-site equilibration rate constant) for sufentanil and alfentanil taken from this reference and for midazolam from Buhrer et al. A value of 0.243 was used for keoof propofol.**

(Figure 2) presents a comparison of MET and decrement time (defined as the time required for the plasma concentration to decrease from C90for surgical anesthesia to C50for recovery) for sufentanil, alfentanil, propofol, and midazolam. There is little difference between decrement time and MET for alfentanil and sufentanil for which delta (for recovery) is 9.2 and 5.99, respectively. In contrast, for propofol and midazolam (delta = 3.28 and 3.054, respectively), there are significant differences between decrement time and MET. The magnitudes of these differences tend to increase with increasing duration of administration, although this difference does reach a plateau for midazolam.

(Figure 3) compares MET calculated from plasma drug concentration and MET calculated by assuming that the probability of drug response should be related to the effect-site concentration. It can be seen that, for the drugs under consideration, the differences between MET sub plasma and METeffect-site are minor.

(Figure 4) compares MET calculated by assuming no pharmacokinetic variability with results using a modified logistic equation that incorporates pharmacokinetic variability in an approximate manner. There are only minor differences between the two methods (less than 15%). It also can be noted that accounting for pharmacokinetic variability may either shorten or lengthen MET, depending on the drug and duration of administration.

(Figure 5) presents MET for the hypothetical case of administration of propofol, with titration to response, to an individual for whom delta = 5.

When a well defined endpoint (e.g., train-of-four recovery) can be identified, the duration of drug effect can be simply calculated as the decrement time to the plasma or effect-site concentration that is associated with that endpoint. However, it is common to describe drug effects as binary, yes or no, variables. In this case, the concentration-effect relationship is defined in terms of probabilities, and it may be difficult to identify an endpoint defining the termination of drug effect. As an example of this difficulty, consider the suggestion of using the decrement time needed to reach the concentration associated with a 5% or 10% probability of drug effect (the 90% or 95% decrement time) as a measure of drug duration. This suggestion seems plausible because, at these concentrations, the patient has only a 5% or 10% probability of drug effect. If the effect-concentration relationship is sharply defined (delta is large), as illustrated by F1-24, this method of defining drug effect duration is meaningful. However, if the concentration-effect relationship is not steep (delta is small), as illustrated in F1-24, this parameter is ambiguous, because at concentrations greater than C5or C10, there may be a substantial cumulative chance that there will be no drug effect. Mean effect time is a method of quantifying the duration of drug effect in this situation.

In this paper, decrement time denotes the time required for the drug concentration to decrease from C90, the concentration used for maintenance of anesthesia in the presence of surgical stimulation, to the C50for recovery (e.g., adequate spontaneous ventilation, responsiveness). As such, it is the median recovery time, i.e., half the patients will recover in less than the decrement time and half will require more time. In contrast, MET is a mean ("average") recovery time. It is based on a "frequentist" interpretation of probability that assumes that an x% probability of an event implies that x% of patients will have the effect and (100 - x)% will not. One might expect the effects of patients requiring less than the decrement time for recovery and of those requiring more than the decrement time for recovery to approximately cancel, so that decrement time and MET would be nearly equal. Indeed, when delta is large, the differences between MET and decrement time are insignificant. However, this occurs because, at concentrations only slightly greater than C50, the probability of drug effect is nearly 1, and at concentrations only slightly less than C50, the probability of drug effect is nearly 0, when delta is large, and thus the probability of effect versus time curve is roughly rectangular, with the border of the rectangle found near the decrement time. In contrast, when delta is relatively small, the distribution of recovery times is skewed to the right, so that the mean recovery time (MET) may be significantly larger than the median recovery time (decrement time;F1-24). In this case, MET is a clinically useful extension of the context-sensitive half-time or decrement time concept, because a drug with a markedly right-skewed distribution of recovery times (large MET) would be undesirable if a subset of patients require a very long time for recovery, even if the median patient recovers quickly (short decrement time).

In most instances, pharmacodynamic analysis has been based on logistic regression of data pooled from multiple patients. To some extent, a flat concentration-effect curve (low delta) reflects interpatient variability. For any individual patient, the concentration-effect relationship may be steep. If so and if the clinician has titrated the drug dose to the specific concentration-effect relationship of the individual patient, the mean effect time concept is not uniquely significant. This is an important clinical point, graphically illustrated in F5-24, which presents MET as a function of the duration of administration of propofol, with titration to response, for the hypothetical case of an individual patient for whom delta = 5. Comparison to F2-24shows that recovery is much faster in this situation. This reflects both the steep concentration-effect curved during recovery and the small decrement between C90and C50when delta is large. It is clear that, if individual concentration-effect curves are steep and if one titrates to effect, very large savings in recovery time may be realized.

The extent to which individual concentration-effect relationships are steep (delta > 5) is unclear. In general, we would expect variation in the drug concentrations associated with specific events during recovery if the drug were administered to the same patient on multiple occasions. In other words, the concentration-effect relationship for an individual patient is probabilistic. Although these individual concentration-effect relationships are often steep for alfentanil, a recent study has shown that, in some patients, delta may be as low as 2.1. There are no data available for other drugs.

In practice, it can be difficult to fully titrate drugs to the individual concentration-effect relationship, and, consequently, intravenous anesthetic agents usually are given within fixed dose ranges. Although opioids may be titrated to hemodynamic response, it is not known whether there is any correlation between the concentrations needed to prevent tachycardia or hypertension during maintenance and the concentrations associated with recovery of adequate spontaneous ventilation. For example, the fact that an individual requires a plasma concentration twice that of the average patient during maintenance of surgical anesthesia does not of necessity mean that he or she will resume adequate spontaneous ventilation at a concentration equal to 2 x C50(for recovery). Also, drugs given for their hypnotic properties are difficult to titrate because we have no direct monitor of awareness. Consequently, anesthetic agents often are not titrated sufficiently during maintenance to predict the specific recovery-phase pharmacodynamics of any individual patient. There may be little correlation between the plasma concentrations that result from the fixed dose range used for maintenance in all patients and the concentration at which the individual patient emerges. Hence, the average recovery time for the population may be predicted best by a mean effect time calculated from data pooled from multiple patients.

MET is a function of C90, C50, delta, and the pharmacokinetic parameters. Strictly, it should be calculated as an average of this function over the distribution of individual values of C sub 90, C50, delta, and pharmacokinetic parameters in the population. This is currently impossible because these distributions are unknown. In this study, MET has been evaluated using the common statistical approximation of replacing an average function of parameters by the function of the average parameters. The accuracy of this approximation cannot be evaluated without further knowledge of the distributions of individual values of C90, C50, and delta.

To calculate the mean effect time, the concentration-effect relationship must be known. This is the major limitation to use of this parameter, because these data are scarce. However, based on the limited data available, some tentative observations can be made. Wessen et al. presented a pharmacodynamic study of emergence in patients who received propofol and either an infusion of alfentanil or a lumbar epidural injection for maintenance of anesthesia. The calculations of MET for propofol reported in this paper are based on their data for the propofol-alfentanil group. However, Wessen et al. report a steeper concentration-effect relationship for the lumbar epidural group than the alfentanil group with delta = 5.66 and 3.28 (in the logistic equation) at extubation, respectively. Referring to F2-24, we see that, for delta = 3.28, there is a significant difference between MET and the decrement time, particularly after longer use. In contrast, for delta = 5.66, MET is indistinguishable from the decrement time. Wessen et al. report a statistically significant prolongation of recovery in the patients receiving alfentanil, which they attribute to a left shift of the concentration-effect curve. However, there was less than a 20% difference in C50between these two groups. It is plausible that the prolongation of recovery may be due to the much larger difference in delta between the two groups, as reflected in the increased MET.

Jacobs et al. published a pharmacodynamic study of midazolam and report that delta = 3.054 with logistic equation analysis of their responsiveness versus concentration data. As seen in F2-24, this implies a significant difference between MET and decrement time.

In contrast to propofol and midazolam, there are only insignificant differences between MET and decrement time for alfentanil and sufentanil. For both of these drugs, delta for recovery is relatively high. However, it should be noted that the decrement time for sufentanil is longer than that for alfentanil, a reversal of the relation of their context-sensitive half-times. This is a reflection of the flat concentration-effect relationship for maintenance of surgical anesthesia with sufentanil reported by Sear and Gavaghan* and used to calculate C90in this paper. Because the concentration-effect relationship is flat, the drug concentration during maintenance must be relatively large to reduce the probability of response to noxious stimuli to 10%. C90is higher in relation to C50for sufentanil than for alfentanil, which has a steep concentration-effect relationship during maintenance of anesthesia. In other words, based on the available pharmacodynamic data, recovery from sufentanil will require a larger decrement (73%) than from alfentanil (56%). This conclusion must be viewed as tentative, because the data on sufentanil pharmacodynamics are relatively sparse and electroencephalographic investigations do not suggest significant differences between the concentration-effect relationships of sufentanil and alfentanil. However, this tentative conclusion underscores the fact that understanding the duration of drug effect requires pharmacodynamic as well as pharmacokinetic data.

Calculation of MET for fentanyl was not done because a complete analysis of the pharmacodynamics of recovery from fentanyl was not found in the literature. Although values of C50have been reported, the analyses do not include a description of the steepness of the concentration-effect relationship, i.e., delta has not reported for recovery from fentanyl. However, the simulations shown in F1-24suggest there will be significant differences between MET and decrement time for fentanyl if delta is low.

MET did not change significantly when calculated by relating the probability of drug response to the effect-site concentration. The differences were comparable to the respective blood effect-site equilibration half-times. However, it should be emphasized that this study did not consider drugs that slowly equilibrate with their effect sites, such as morphine. Furthermore, the study did not consider drugs that have active metabolites.

The effect of pharmacokinetic variability was incorporated into the calculations using a well known mathematical technique, the truncated Taylor's series. This leads to an expression for the probability of drug effect, which is a linear function of the variance of the drug concentration. To calculate MET, one must have a mathematical representation of this variance. In this study, the concentration variance was calculated using the simple constant coefficient of variation model, varC= sigma2C2. With this model, the change in MET due to pharmacokinetic variability will be proportional to the constant delta2. In pharmacokinetic studies using the constant coefficient of variation model, sigma2is equal to the mean squared prediction error. The simulations shown in F4-24were performed with sigma2= 0.2, which is somewhat larger than reported values. This should tend to magnify the effects of pharmacokinetic variability. Despite this, F4-24indicates that pharmacokinetic variability had little effect on MET. It should be noted that the MET concept provides a framework for evaluating the effect of pharmacokinetic variability on context-sensitive half-times, because in the limit of increasing delta, MET and context-sensitive half-time are equal. MET was evaluated for fentanyl, sufentanil, alfentanil, midazolam, and propofol for delta = 20 and C50=(0.5) C90. In this case, MET is equal to context-sensitive half-time for all of these drugs. Adding pharmacokinetic variability with sigma2= 0.2 had an insignificant effect on the value of MET (less than a 10% difference). It appears that, although pharmacodynamic variability tends to skew the distribution of recovery times to the right, pharmacokinetic variability has a symmetric effect on the distribution, such that the mean recovery time, MET, is not significantly altered. However, this conclusion must be viewed as tentative until the results of pharmacokinetic analysis that more accurately model pharmacokinetic variability are incorporated into the calculation of MET.

The applicability of the MET concept to drugs whose effects can be measured in a continuous manner is unclear. If there is a level of effect that can be accepted as a clinical endpoint, the pertinent measure of drug duration is simply the time needed for the drug concentration to decline from maintenance levels to the level associated with this endpoint. In this case, the area under the effect versus time curve is not relevant, whereas the decrement time to this endpoint is the appropriate measure of duration of effect.

The results of this study demonstrate the importance of complete pharmacodynamic characterization of drugs. Although determination of single points on the concentration-effect curve, i.e., C50, is obviously important, a complete analysis of the drug requires an understanding of the shape of the curve, often embodied in the parameter delta. Unfortunately, these latter data sometimes are not evaluated or reported.

In summary, MET, as defined in this paper, is a parameter that extends the context-sensitive half-time/decrement time concept to use both pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic data to quantify the duration of drug effect when the effect is a binary, response/no response variable. When the concentration-effect relationship is steep, MET equals the relevant decrement time. However, there can be significant differences between MET and decrement times when there is a flatter concentration-effect relationship. Based on available data, MET for propofol (when used in conjunction with alfentanil) and midazolam is significantly greater than decrement time, and MET for sufentanil and alfentanil is nearly equal to the respective decrement time. The inclusion of pharmacokinetic variability into the calculation seems to have little effect on MET.

Calculation of MET using the logistic equation

formula

implicitly assumes that the drug concentration at any time is the same for all patients. To take pharmacokinetic variability into effect, note that the probability of drug effect, P, can be expressed in a Taylor's series expansion around the mean concentration in the population, <C> as follows:

formula

. + higher order terms. In this equation,

formula

and the derivatives dP/dC and d2P/dC2are evaluated at C = <C>. The probability of drug effect, averaged (in the sense of a mean) over the population and ignoring higher order terms, is then;

formula

since

formula

and

formula

by definition. Because MET is a mean parameter, pharmacokinetic variability is accounted for by calculating MET using M2-24A. It is a straightforward exercise in differentiation to show that

formula

. This term is denoted K in the text.

* Sear JW, Gavaghan D: Dynamic-kinetic relationships for sufentanil during nitrous oxide-oxygen anaesthesia (abstract). Br J Anaesth 65:290P-291P, 1990.

** Shafer SL: Private communication.

1.
Shafer SL, Varvel JR: Pharmacokinetics, pharmacodynamics, and rational opioid selection. ANESTHESIOLOGY 74:53-63, 1991.
2.
Hughes MA, Glass PSA, Jacobs JR: Context-sensitive half-time in multicompartment pharmacokinetic models for intravenous anesthetic drugs. ANESTHESIOLOGY 76:334-341, 1992.
3.
Youngs EJ, Shafer SL: Pharmacokinetic parameters relevant to recovery from opioids. ANESTHESIOLOGY 81:833-842, 1994.
4.
Kern SE: Determining recovery times from pharmacological effect (abstract). ANESTHESIOLOGY 81:A403, 1994.
5.
Stanski DR: Monitoring depth of anesthesia, Anesthesia. 4th edition. Edited by Miller RD. New York, Churchill Livingstone, 1994, pp 1135-1138.
6.
Wessen A, Persson PM, Nilsson A, Hartvig P: Concentration-effect relationship of propofol after total intravenous anesthesia. Anesth Analg 77:1000-1007, 1993.
7.
Jacobs JR, Reves JF, Marty J, White WD, Bai SA, Smith LR: Aging increases pharmacodynamic sensitivity to the hypnotic effects of midazolam. Anesth Analg 80:143-148, 1995.
8.
Ausems ME, Hug CC Jr, Stanski DR, Burm AGL: Plasma concentrations of alfentanil required to supplement nitrous oxide anesthesia for general surgery. ANESTHESIOLOGY 65:362-373, 1986.
9.
Schwilden H, Schuttler J, Stoekel H: Pharmacokinetics as applied to total intravenous anaesthesia: Theoretical considerations. Anaesthesia 38:51-52, 1983.
10.
Shafer SL, Varvel JR, Aziz N, Scott JC: Pharmacokinetics of fentanyl administered by computer-controlled infusion pump. ANESTHESIOLOGY 73:1091-1102, 1990.
11.
Buhrer M, Maitre PO, Crevoisier C, Stanski DR: Electroencephalographic effects of benzodiazepines: II. Pharmacodynamic modeling of the electroencephalographic effects of midazolam and diazepam. Clin Pharmacol Ther 48:555-567, 1990.
12.
Vuyk J, Lim T, Engbers FHM, Burm AGL, Vletter AA, Bovill JG: Pharmacodynamics of alfentanil as supplement to propofol or nitrous oxide for lower abdominal surgery in female patients. ANESTHESIOLOGY 78:1036-1045, 1993.
13.
Dyck JB, Maze M, Haack C, Azornoff DL, Vuorilehto L, Shafer SL: Computer-controlled infusion of intravenous dexmedetomidine hydrochloride in adult human volunteers. ANESTHESIOLOGY 78:821-828, 1993.
14.
Egan TD, Lemmens HJM, Fiset P, Herman DJ, Muir KT, Stanski DR: The pharmacokinetics of the new short-acting opioid remifentanil (GI87084B) in healthy adult male volunteers. ANESTHESIOLOGY 79: 881-892, 1993.
15.
Kataria BK, Ved SA, Nicodemus HF, Hoy GR, Lea D, Dubois MY, Mandema JW, Shafer SL: The pharmacokinetics of propofol in children using three different data analysis approaches. ANESTHESIOLOGY 80:104-122, 1994.
16.
Sheiner LB, Beal SL: Evaluation of methods for estimating population pharmacokinetic parameters; II. Biexponential model and experimental pharmacokinetic data. J Pharmacokin Biopharm 9:635-651, 1981.
17.
Fisher DM: Propofol in pediatrics: Lessons in pharmacokinetic modeling. ANESTHESIOLOGY 80:2-5, 1994.