Dennis M. Fisher, M.D., Editor

In 1992, we published a review article in Anesthesiology on learning and consciousness during general anesthesia. [1] Since then, there has been a steady flow of publications and two international conferences on the subject; one in 1992 in Atlanta, Georgia, and another in 1995 in Rotterdam, the Netherlands. In 1992, the issue of preservation of unconscious information-processing capability by the anesthetized patient was debated because of the mixed results of studies at that time. We and others [2,3] thought that with improvement in the quality of work in this area, the issue of whether unconscious retention of intraoperative events is a reliable, replicable phenomenon might be resolved in a few years. Unfortunately, this has not happened. With conflicting reports and contradictory statements being published, we chose to update the reader on this and related subjects with particular emphasis on problems and difficulties in this area, and to provide some comments that we hope will be useful for future studies.

Here we address the following issues.

1. Explicit or conscious memory or “awareness”;

2. Studies of anesthetized patients using implicit tests of memory;

3. Postoperative motor behavior after presentation of suggestions during anesthesia;

4. Studies of the efficacy of administration of therapeutic suggestions during anesthesia;

5. Studies of healthy volunteers;

6. Effects of anesthetics on animal learning and memory; and

7. Reports of cases of psychosomatic disorders after anesthesia and surgery.

In each area, we discuss in particular the studies that have been published since 1992, within the context of the earlier literature, followed by comments and critiques. We use the term consciousness as defining a state of awareness of the outside world and use the terms consciousness, awareness, and wakefulness interchangeably.

Distinctions between different types of learning and memory remain the subject of considerable debate. One such distinction, of particular interest to anesthesia, is between explicit and implicit learning. The essential difference is whether learning is manifested with or without concurrent awareness of remembering. [4,5]

Incidence 

The most recent study in which a structured interview was used in a large series of patients indicated that the incidence in nonobstetric and noncardiac surgical cases is 0.2%. [6] The incidence is similar after total intravenous anesthesia. [7] It is higher, however, when light anesthesia is used. The incidence in cardiac surgery ranges from 1.14–1.5%, [8,9] with a balanced anesthetic technique consisting of benzodiazepines, low-dose fentanyl, and a volatile agent. A higher incidence has been reported for obstetric cases [10](0.4%) and major trauma cases [11](11–43%), and this incidence varies according to the dose of anesthetic administered. Jones [12] estimates that only about 0.01% of patients report suffering from pain while being aware.

Consequences 

Recently Cobcroft and Forsdick [13] and Moerman et al. [14] studied the consequences of recall of intraoperative events during anesthesia. Cobcroft and Forsdick described patients who responded to an article published by Forsdick about her own experience of awareness during anesthesia in a lay magazine widely distributed in Australia and New Zealand. Moerman et al. studied patients who were referred to them by anesthesiologists. Despite the different strategies of patient selection of the two groups, there was close agreement between the results. The two most frequent complaints were ability to hear events during surgery and sensations of weakness or paralysis, in addition to the recall of pain, if it was present. It seems that patients particularly recall conversations or remarks that are of a negative nature concerning themselves or their medical conditions. The most frequently reported postoperative effects were sleep disturbances, dreams and nightmares, flashbacks, and daytime anxiety.

For many patients, the experience of awareness may not leave prolonged after effects; however, some develop post-traumatic stress disorder, marked by repetitive nightmares (usually poorly disguised replays of an operative situation), anxiety and irritability, a preoccupation with death, and a concern with sanity, that make the patients reluctant to discuss their symptoms. [15] It is not readily apparent why some patients develop a post-traumatic stress disorder and others do not.

The medicolegal consequences of awareness remain of interest. Domino* recently analyzed claims from the American Society of Anesthesiologists Closed Claims Project. Claims for awareness during anesthesia were 2% of all claims. This incidence was similar to rates of claims for such familiar complications after anesthesia such as aspiration pneumonia and myocardial infarction. Female gender tripled the likelihood of an awareness claim, compared with other general anesthesia malpractice claims. Domino speculated that women may be more likely than men to sue for emotional injury.

Causes and Prevention 

The precise concentration of anesthetic agent required to guarantee lack of recall is unknown. Minimal values of 0.8–1.0 minimum alveolar concentration of inhalation anesthetics currently appear acceptable. The effects of adding intravenous agents, such as benzodiazepines, propofol, and opioids, to inhalation anesthetics, as is sometimes done in clinical practice, remain to be studied. Inspection of the anesthetic records of awareness cases for relevant parameters such as heart rate, blood pressure, and anesthetic technique has not been helpful in retrospectively explaining why awareness or recall occurred. [8,14] Moerman et al. [14] also found that 65% of patients who experienced awareness and recall during general anesthesia did not inform their anesthesiologists about what happened. Asking the patient four simple questions-What was the last thing you remember before you went to sleep? What was the first thing you remember when you woke up? Can you remember anything in between these two periods? and Did you dream during your operation?-should be part of the anesthesiologists' postoperative interview and should allow the anesthesiologist to deal with this traumatic experience at an appropriate early time. Prompt referral to a qualified therapist and acknowledgment of what has happened yields the best chance for recovery.** Cobcroft and Forsdick [13] concluded that, in most cases of awareness, understanding of the phenomenon and its management by medical personnel were poor or entirely lacking.

Conclusions 

Conscious recall of intraoperative events is relatively rare and development of post-traumatic stress disorder is even more uncommon. However, when we consider that approximately 20 million general anesthetics are administered each year in the United States, a 0.2% incidence corresponds to 40,000 cases of awareness annually. It is probable that the incidence of this complication of anesthesia has reached a plateau and that further significant decreases depend on avoidance of “overly” light anesthetic techniques; gaining more knowledge about anesthetic requirements of patients; development of methods to detect consciousness during anesthesia [1]; and disseminating information to anesthesiologists and their acceptance [9] of the virtues of vigilance and rational use of muscle relaxants (i.e., avoiding muscle paralysis unless it is needed for intubation or surgery and even then avoiding total paralysis if possible).

Future insights into the epidemiologic characteristics and prevention of post-traumatic stress disorder associated with awareness, which may cause long-lasting devastating damage to patients' lives, are needed. Because of the low incidence, a multiple-center study is necessary, if such data are to be obtained.

An Awareness Monitor? 

The concept of an awareness monitor that would track patients' arousal levels and warn of impending wakefulness is popular. Monitoring the mid-latency auditory-evoked responses [12] or the bispectral index of the electroencephalograph [16] seems promising as an indicator of loss of consciousness and its unintended return during surgery. However, it remains for future work to identify some parameter that unambiguously defines when consciousness is lost. Although this may be feasible and a monitor could possibly be developed and adapted for clinical practice, two questions remain to be answered: Do we need such a monitor?, and Can we afford it? The answers are likely to be far from certain or unanimous. Proponents for the development of such a monitor will probably refer to anesthesiologists' inability to determine reliably whether a given anesthetized and paralyzed patient is conscious during surgery. They will point to medical malpractice claims resulting from consciousness and recall during general anesthesia, the high incidence of wakefulness without recall that has been reported in some studies, [17–20] and the possible psychic trauma that may result from the unconscious storage of a traumatic event. [15,21] They may indicate that we currently have monitors for the actions of anesthetic drugs on the cardiovascular and respiratory systems and the neuromuscular junction but none for their actions on the brain, their primary target. Such a monitor also may help to ensure protection of the brain from injury under certain circumstances. Finally, such a monitor would be valuable in determining the role of consciousness in what is presumed to be implicit learning and memory (explained in the next section). This would have important clinical and theoretical implications. Could not the costs of a new monitor be justified by better outcome for the patients and shorter lengths of stay in the recovery room by improved intraoperative anesthetic management?

Opponents of developing such a monitor would argue that consciousness and explicit recall of events during surgery are relatively rare. Because most cases occur from error by the anesthesiologist, [1] the most effective method of reducing their incidence is likely to relate to improved standards of care by anesthesiologists rather than dependence on a monitor. [22] A shorter stay in the operating room and in the recovery room through improved anesthetic management might not decrease costs. [23,24] The costs incurred for research and development of the monitor, which eventually will be passed on to the consumer, may exceed its benefits, and its purchase will have a low priority compared with other necessary current and future health care expenditures.

Explicit versus Implicit Memory 

Explicit memory refers to intentional or conscious recollection of prior experiences as assessed by tests of recall or recognition. Implicit memory, by contrast, refers to changes in performance or behavior that are produced by prior experiences on tests that do not require any intentional or conscious recollection of those experiences. [25] The basic distinction between explicit or direct tests and implicit or indirect tests involves the nature of the instructions given to the person being tested. In a direct test, participants are asked to recall or recognize events that may have occurred during anesthesia. In an indirect test, the instructions make no reference to events during the operation. There is now evidence that performance on both direct and indirect tests may reflect both conscious and unconscious memory processes. [26,27] In particular, indirect tests are prone to influence by explicit memory if participants become aware of the relationship between study and test items and exploit this knowledge. [28]

Types of Memory Tests 

There are various types of indirect or implicit memory tests. Some types may be more sensitive for detecting evidence of retention of events occurring during anesthesia. Table 1lists the studies that used indirect tests of memory and identifies the anesthetic drugs used for maintenance of anesthesia, whether memory for information that was presented during anesthesia was found, and some brief comments. The task of recognition is usually construed as a measure of explicit memory, a view based on studies with patients with amnesia, whose performance on the task is usually impaired. [5] An alternative view, based on studies of healthy persons, is that recognition depends on both explicit and implicit memories. [27,56] According to Mandler, [56] two simultaneous processes are involved when a person must decide whether he or she recognizes an item. First, the “familiarity” of the item is retrieved; second, a search process determines whether the item was presented previously. The first process may fall within the domain of implicit memory. The success of some researchers with forced-choice recognition tasks in anesthetized patients (Table 1) may support this contention, assuming that patients are guessing and are guided by feelings of familiarity. Therefore we included this task in the table.

Table 1. Studies with Indirect Tests of Memory 

Table 1. Studies with Indirect Tests of Memory 
Table 1. Studies with Indirect Tests of Memory 

Table 5.  Table 1 (continued). Studies with Indirect Tests of Memory 

Table 5.  Table 1(continued). Studies with Indirect Tests of Memory 
Table 5.  Table 1(continued). Studies with Indirect Tests of Memory 

An unforced recognition task, which allows participants not to respond if they are unsure, may be less sensitive to implicit memory than a recognition task in which participants are forced to respond to every test item, even if they believe they are only guessing, because guesses may reflect implicit memory. [75] Several groups of investigators found evidence for memory during anesthesia using forced-choice recognition tasks. [3,46,48,49,55]

Other tasks have been used with mixed results. An exception is the word-completion task, for which the results have been positive. However, this task, has been used only by two groups of investigators. [31,55]

Other variables in the studies that may influence their outcome include the anesthetic technique, the salience of the experimental stimuli, the number of stimulus presentations, the time interval between presentation and the test, and sample size.

Comments and Critiques 

We could argue that any learning during general anesthesia is implicit, because the patient is rendered unconscious by the general anesthetic drugs. Skeptics may, however, counter that there is no reliable way to determine adequacy of anesthesia, particularly in the paralyzed patient. The interaction between the administered anesthetic doses and varying levels of surgical stimulation may lead to episodes of awareness. In addition, as we discuss later, explicit recall is abolished before loss of responsiveness, so it is possible for a patient to be responsive and conscious during surgery and yet not remember this after recovery. Therefore appropriate performance on indirect tests of memory after anesthesia may not necessarily imply the presence of unconscious learning and implicit memory during anesthesia. [26,58] Conceivably, an implicit test might show memory while an explicit test does not show memory because the former test is more sensitive than the latter, rather than because it engages a memory system different than the explicit one.

Therefore it would be of interest to use procedures that may provide uncontaminated estimates of conscious and unconscious processes. Caseley-Rondi et al. [49] recommend two such approaches, “relatively sensitivity” and “qualitative differences,” that they assert might provide more rigorous evidence of unconscious learning during anesthesia.

The “relative sensitivity” approach uses explicit and implicit tests that are as comparable as possible in all respects except whether they overtly request recollection of previously presented information. Under these circumstances, if participants show more memory on the implicit test than the explicit test, unconscious learning can be inferred. If the explicit and implicit tests differ in other respects, according to Caseley-Rondi et al., [49] these other differences might be responsible for any differences in performance and would complicate an interpretation in terms of unconscious learning. Unconscious learning during anesthesia has not yet been definitively demonstrated in a manner satisfying Caseley-Rondi et al.'s requirement, because the two studies using the most similar explicit and implicit tests-forced-choice recognition and preference judgments, respectively-did not demonstrate differential performance on these tests. [31,49]

The “qualitative differences” approach [49] uses a test in which conscious and unconscious learning should produce qualitatively different (i.e., opposite) effects on performance. For example, using a procedure developed by Jacoby et al., [59] participants might be presented with nonfamous names such as John Schultz during anesthesia. After operation, they would be presented with these names, mixed together with other nonfamous names and famous names, and asked to indicate whether each name is famous or not. They would be told before the test that all the names presented during anesthesia were not famous. Conscious retention of presentation of the name John Schultz during anesthesia should, therefore, lead to classifying it as nonfamous, whereas unconscious retention might lead to classifying it as famous.

Application of the relative sensitivity and qualitative differences approaches may lead to more definitive demonstrations of unconscious learning during anesthesia. However, such methods may not appeal to all investigators. Tests that do not fit within these approaches may have other virtues; for example, they may prove more sensitive to learning during anesthesia or be more realistic or meaningful. It remains appropriate for investigators to use other types of tests to study learning during anesthesia, and, perhaps, to assume tentatively that learning detected by these tests is unconscious, provided that this assumption is consistent with patients' self-reports and anesthesia seems to be adequate; that is, patients cannot recollect hearing anything during anesthesia and they have no concept that a relationship exists between material that was presented during anesthesia and their performance on an incidental, seemingly “nonmemory” test. If patients have no concept of such a relationship, they cannot be aware that the presented material influences their performance on the test.

Influence of Anesthetic Regimens on Outcomes of Studies. Because some benzodiazepines impair implicit memory, [60–62] it is prudent to avoid their use in studies of implicit memory during anesthesia. Block et al. [31] found that the anesthetic technique-nitrous oxide with opioids or with isoflurane-did not affect performance on the implicit memory tests that were used. However, an influence of anesthesia method could have been missed because of the small sample size. Kihlstrom et al. [41] found evidence for implicit memory during anesthesia with isoflurane but no implicit memory in a second study [42] in which they used nitrous oxide and sufentanil. Schwender et al. [44] found that in patients with implicit memory after operation, the midlatency auditory-evoked potentials during anesthesia continued to show a pattern similar to the awake state, but in contrast, in the patients without implicit memory, the waveforms were severely attenuated or abolished. Most of the patients in the first group were anesthetized with flunitrazepam and fentanyl and most of the patients in the second group were anesthetized with isoflurane and fentanyl or propofol and fentanyl. This may suggest that implicit memory is possible mainly under light anesthesia. The results of Jelicic et al., [37] in which they failed to replicate their earlier demonstration of learning [36] when they used nitrous oxide and enflurane instead of nitrous oxide and opioids, are consistent with this suggestion. The contradictory results of Kihlstrom et al. [41,42] may represent failure of replication of an earlier study rather than a difference caused by the two anesthetic methods.

Jelicic and Bonke, [63] in an attempt to explain the effects of different anesthetic techniques on implicit learning during anesthesia (Table 1), hypothesized that single words can be activated in postoperative implicit memory during both nitrous oxide anesthesia and anesthesia with potent volatile agents. However, learning of more complex information may be only possible in patients during nitrous oxide anesthesia, alone or supplemented with opioids. Whatever the explanation, it seems that, in the absence of a monitor for the depth of anesthesia, investigators should standardize the anesthetic methods used as closely as possible with respect to drugs and their dosages.

Types of Information Presented and Tested. Many implicit memory tasks can be categorized as perceptual or conceptual. [64] Perceptual memory tests can be exemplified by the word completion task. The beneficial effect of study on test performance has been labeled perceptual priming because the cue in these tests specifies the perceptual form of the studied stimulus word. In contrast, an implicit test may provide information that is conceptually related to the studied information but with no apparent perceptual similarity between the study and test stimuli. Examples of such tasks include answering general knowledge questions or generating category exemplars. The increased likelihood that participants say or write the target word if it was presented at study has been labeled conceptual priming. An unambiguous classification of priming tasks according to whether they depend on perceptual or conceptual priming remains elusive, however. Carlesimo, [65] for example, presented evidence that both perceptual and conceptual processes may contribute to priming in word-completion tests. It has been suggested [66] that anesthetized patients may perform better on perceptual rather than conceptual tasks, but there is insufficient evidence to confirm this suggestion.

Another distinction pertinent to implicit memory concerns locating and activating information that already exists in memory (e.g., a representation in memory of a word that is already part of a person's vocabulary) versus forming a new representation in memory (e.g., learning a new word and storing in memory some kind of cognitive structure that represents that word). [67] There are two important differences between implicit memory for “old”(already existing) and new representations. [68] First, the creation of new representations in memory may require the participant to engage in some mental activities that are not necessary for locating and activating old representations. Second, implicit memory for new information is difficult to observe in severely amnesic patients. Kihlstrom and Schacter [68] have, therefore, hypothesized that implicit memory for events during anesthesia might be confined to the activation of preexisting knowledge. However, the study of Block et al. [31] using nonsense words and Jelicic et al. [36] using fictitious nonfamous names suggest that new representations or associations may be formed in memory during anesthesia.

Familiarity of Stimuli. The issue of familiarity of the material used in the memory tests is also of interest. So, too, is the material's associative frequency or dominance (i.e., the probability with which a stimulus evokes a response). For example, if participants are asked to name examples of the category “metal,” some examples with high associative frequency or dominance are given frequently (such as iron), whereas other examples with low associative frequency or dominance are given infrequently (such as tungsten). Jelicic et al. [30] and Roorda-Hrdlickova et al. [29] tested patients with a category-generation task using familiar categories and familiar target words. They obtained evidence for learning during anesthesia. However, Bonebakker et al. [34] from the same research group found no such evidence when they used less familiar target words. Bonebakker et al. [55] found that, using the word-completion task, patients showed more evidence of priming for words with high associative frequency or dominance compared with words with lesser ratings. Results from studies with persons with and without amnesia show that less familiar words result in larger priming effects. [69,70] This may not be the case in anesthetized patients. Bonebakker et al. [55] suggested that familiarity with the information to which patients are exposed during anesthesia is important for successful priming.

Number of Stimulus Presentations. In conscious persons, repetition and duration of exposure to the stimulus seem to enhance conceptual, but not perceptual, priming. [71,72] Recently, Bonebakker et al. [55] found that memory during anesthesia was apparent after one, but not after 30 presentations of a word list. An explanation that has been offered for these results [55] is that patients may unconsciously associate the presented material with an aversive period during surgery. Brown et al. [32] found that performance on indirect memory tests was worse for words that were presented three times during anesthesia than for words that were not presented at all. The investigators suggested that memory may be suppressed by the aversive experience of surgery. However, this somewhat implausible speculation stands in contrast to several studies that showed memory effects after multiple presentations. [31,36,41] Brown et al.'s [32] statistical analyses of their results were also questionable. Because the effect was only marginal in the primary analysis, follow-up analyses were not warranted. The analyses were also based on pooling the results of two entirely separate types of tests.

Time of Postoperative Testing. Priming effects (i.e., hearing the words on the list increases participants' ability to identify with the correct solutions when they respond to the test) were found in anesthetized patients when they were tested very early after operation (3–5 h after surgery)[30] or as late as 5 days after surgery. [44] The optimal time of testing after operation could be influenced by several factors. Patients should be tested when they have recovered sufficiently from the detrimental effects of anesthetics on cognitive function that could impair their performance, but before priming effects have dissipated. Durations of the priming effects may vary according to the task used. [67] Andrade [73] suggests that testing should be conducted in the hospital to minimize changes in context between stimulus presentation and the memory test, although it remains to be demonstrated that context effects significantly influence performance on indirect tests of memory. Extraneous stimuli may also interfere with the specific stimuli administered during anesthesia, if the testing is delayed. Merikle and Daneman [74] recently conducted a meta-analysis of studies investigating memory for events during anesthesia. Memory decreased systematically as the interval between the end of surgery and the administration of the memory test increased, and there seemed to be no memory when testing was delayed more than 36 h after surgery.

In contrast, consolidation of implicit memory, active during sleep, depends strongly on rapid eye movement sleep. [75] Sleep pattern after surgery is severely disturbed with early depression of rapid eye movement and slow wave sleep and with rebound of rapid eye movement sleep on the second and third nights. [76] Conceivably, this might favor testing on the third postoperative day.

Sample Size. In studies providing evidence of learning during anesthesia, the effects usually have been small. [31,41,46] This is consistent with the possibility that only a minority of persons can learn during anesthesia, perhaps due to a relatively light level of anesthesia. [77] Positive findings seem more likely to be replicated in studies with substantial samples of patients, which provide adequate power to detect relatively small effects. Studies with small samples, such as 20–30 patients, may lead to conflicting results.

Bennett et al. [78] introduced this method in 1985. Initially, three groups of investigators reported positive results, [31,78,79] despite criticisms of absence of baseline assessment of the target behavior, presence of too light a level of anesthesia during presentation of the suggestions, and deficiencies in the statistical analyses. Later reports were negative both in anesthetized patients and in healthy volunteers treated with subanesthetic concentrations of inhalation anesthetics. [51,80–83]

In addition to providing evidence of learning during anesthesia, the possibility of improving the postoperative course of patients by presentation during anesthesia of therapeutic suggestions predicting a rapid and comfortable postoperative recovery is an attractive clinical goal. Table 2lists the studies that have been done in this area.

Table 2. Therapeutic Suggestions Studies 

Table 2. Therapeutic Suggestions Studies 
Table 2. Therapeutic Suggestions Studies 

Table 6.  Table 2 (continued). Therapeutic Suggestions Studies 

Table 6.  Table 2(continued). Therapeutic Suggestions Studies 
Table 6.  Table 2(continued). Therapeutic Suggestions Studies 

Hospital Stay 

Evans and Richardson [48] reported that patients who received therapeutic suggestions during anesthesia for hysterectomy had shorter postoperative stays than did a nonsuggestion control group. In contrast, Liu et al. [93] could not find positive effects. Millar [77] conducted a meta-analysis of the results of Evans and Richardson and Liu et al. He concluded that Evans and Richardson's results may have been caused by chance bias in allocation of patients to the control group as a result of a relatively small sample size (Figure 1). Jelicic et al. [95] exposed patients to both affirmative ('you will be comfortable") and nonaffirmative ("you will have no pain") suggestions, affirmative or nonaffirmative suggestions separately, or some irrelevant text. They obtained somewhat contradictory and confusing findings: Patients who received both affirmative and nonaffirmative suggestions spent less time in the hospital than did patients in the other three groups. The authors acknowledged the possibility that the outcome might have been due to chance.

Figure 1. Distribution of patient data points (days to discharge) in the two groups of Evans and Richardson (1988) in London and three groups of Liu et al. (1992) in Nottingham. Test groups were presented with therapeutic suggestions during anesthesia. The suffixes “B” and “H” indicate that those control groups received a blank or “history” tape, respectively, in the Nottingham study. With the presentation of data in scatter plots, it is evident that, although four of the groups were similar in their mean and typical durations of stay in the hospital, the London control group was distinctive, with most patients staying longer in the hospital. (Reproduced with permission from Millar. [77])

Figure 1. Distribution of patient data points (days to discharge) in the two groups of Evans and Richardson (1988) in London and three groups of Liu et al. (1992) in Nottingham. Test groups were presented with therapeutic suggestions during anesthesia. The suffixes “B” and “H” indicate that those control groups received a blank or “history” tape, respectively, in the Nottingham study. With the presentation of data in scatter plots, it is evident that, although four of the groups were similar in their mean and typical durations of stay in the hospital, the London control group was distinctive, with most patients staying longer in the hospital. (Reproduced with permission from Millar. [77])

Close modal

Postoperative Pain and Analgesic Use 

Korunka et al. [94] found that suggestions or music prolonged the period before patients asked for their first postoperative analgesic, compared with a control tape of operating room sounds, but only the music tape reduced total analgesic consumption. Caseley-Rondi et al. [49] also reported reduction in analgesic requirements in patients who were played therapeutic suggestions during anesthesia. However, differences between the experimental and control groups were only marginally significant in analyses controlling for the greater age of the patients and longer duration of anesthesia (and presumably larger doses of intraoperative opioids) in the experimental group. Furthermore, the significance level in the analysis of morphine use after operation was not adjusted for the number of measures of postoperative recovery that were assessed, increasing the probability of a type 1 error. Van der Laan et al. [99] also found that therapeutic suggestions had no effect on postoperative analgesic requirements.

Nausea and Vomiting 

Williams et al. [96] reported that presentation of positive intraoperative suggestions reduced patients' recalled incidence of vomiting in the first 24 h after surgery by 37% compared with a control group. Patients who received therapeutic suggestions also required smaller doses of metoclopramide. Oddby-Muhrbeck et al. [98] more recently failed to find positive effects on postoperative nausea and vomiting, although therapeutic suggestions did reduce patients' recall of these distressing postoperative symptoms. Differences between these two studies with respect to dependent variables, contents of the therapeutic suggestions tapes, postoperative antiemetic medications, and types of surgery could have contributed to the different conclusions that were reached.

Smoking 

Hughes et al. [97] presented smokers during surgery with a tape encouraging them to give up smoking or a control tape. Compared with patients who received the control tape, more of those who received the suggestions tape had stopped or reduced their smoking according to self-reports 1 month after operation. However, Myles et al. [100] failed to confirm this finding. The strength of the findings of Hughes et al. [97] as evidence of implicit learning during anesthesia is limited, because of several factors. Although no patients recalled the message, their memory was not tested until 1 month after operation; however, some might have recalled it had their memory been probed earlier. The assessment of postoperative smoking was not based on objective records of cigarette smoking or on self-reports obtained at frequent intervals, but only on a single, delayed self-report covering a 1-month period. The validity of such a self-report, especially for patients who reported decreased smoking rather than complete abstention, is uncertain.

Hypnotic Ability 

The interaction of patients' hypnotic ability with the outcome of presentation of therapeutic suggestions has been addressed by two groups of investigators, Caseley-Rondi et al. [49] and Korunka et al. [94] Hypnotic ability was not significantly associated with therapeutic outcome. The two groups also raised the issue of what mediates the therapeutic effect of suggestions. They suggest that it is possible that the soothing tones of a voice rather than semantic processing of connected discourse may be important. Soothing tones, by reducing stress, might aid recovery. The beneficial effect of music in the Korunka et al. study [94] may be consistent with such an explanation and suggests that evidence for postoperative benefit does not necessarily imply that memory is involved.

Critiques and Conclusions 

Statistical Concerns. Andrade and Munglani [101] criticized many studies reporting beneficial effects of presentation of therapeutic suggestions during anesthesia on the grounds that some of these effects might have been due to chance, because the studies assessed multiple measures of recovery without controlling statistically for the number of variables analyzed. When many variables are analyzed and a statistical method such as Bonferroni's method is used to control for the number of variables, an effect must be rather large to be deemed statistically significant. Because beneficial effects of presentation of therapeutic suggestions may not be large, judicious selection while planning a study of a smaller set of dependent variables may be preferable.

Methodologic Concerns. Millar [77] made useful suggestions for improving the methods used. The first suggestion concerns the sample size. Considerably larger sample sizes than the usual samples of 20 or fewer are required, considering the likely possibility that only a few patients in a given sample may be in a state to register auditory information during anesthesia and the sensitivity of the assay. The second suggestion concerns the mode of presentation of the results. Millar suggests that rather than presenting the data exclusively in the summary form of means and standard deviations, better insights into treatment effects may be gained by presenting full data sets in graphic form (Figure 1).

Measures of postoperative well-being and recovery are affected by complex sets of factors that may not be controlled by the experimenter and that may lead to bias when assessing their interactions with presentations of therapeutic suggestions.

Hospital Stay. It appears that the length of hospital stay is not influenced by presentation of therapeutic suggestions to anesthetized patients, [77] and there does not seem to be a good reason anymore to focus on this measure of postoperative recovery. This measure is likely to be affected by factors that are usually beyond the experimenters' control.

Postoperative Pain and Analgesic Use. Although patient-controlled analgesia provides a better measure of patients' requirements of opioids than drugs prescribed on an as-needed basis by physicians and delivered by nurses, some pitfalls may confound this measure. One issue is that the dose and timing of administration of preoperative and intraoperative opioids may reduce postoperative patient-controlled analgesic consumption. [102,103] Another issue is sample size. A simulation study suggested that detecting a 25% decrease of analgesic use between control and intervention groups (the effect size that has been reported in studies of therapeutic suggestions with positive results) requires a sample size of 116 patients. [104] This suggests that none of the studies of therapeutic suggestions had large enough sample sizes.

Nausea and Vomiting. The cause of nausea and vomiting is also multifactorial; that is, it is influenced by type of surgery; anesthetic agents and techniques; patient sex, age, and weight; duration of anesthesia; experience of the anesthesiologist; and history of postoperative nausea and vomiting, motion sickness, middle ear disease, pain, and so on. [105,106] Authors should report these factors and specify whether they were equally represented in patients receiving therapeutic suggestions and controls. This has not been the case.

Conclusions. It is, therefore, apparent that therapeutic suggestions tasks are particularly liable to both type I and type II errors. This is probably the reason for inconsistent results in this area. In addition, even if some patients register the auditory information that is presented, it may not influence their behavior. A recent meta-analysis of studies in this area by Merikle and Daneman [74] found little or no effect of therapeutic suggestions on postoperative recovery.

One of the main aims for work in this area is to determine the concentrations of anesthetics that prevent learning and memory. “Light” anesthesia is commonly used to avoid toxicity or delayed recovery, or because of intolerance of some patients to higher concentrations. Yet the precise concentrations that guarantee absence of recall are unknown. In addition, largely unknown has been whether different anesthetics equally affect different types of memory. These objectives can be achieved by studying complete dose-response functions of acquisition and retention, studying minimum alveolar concentration-equivalent concentrations of different inhalation anesthetics, equipotent plasma concentrations of intravenous anesthetics, and assessments of the degrees of memory impairments in different types of memory tasks. Other related aims are to investigate the effects of sex and aging and the interactions of anesthetics with one another and with arousing stimulations. All these factors are important for clinical application to patients. It should be remembered, however, that the arousing effect of surgery and emotionally charged information may increase the anesthetic concentrations required to prevent consciousness and memory. Table 3summarizes the literature.

Table 3. Studies of Healthy Volunteers 

Table 3. Studies of Healthy Volunteers 
Table 3. Studies of Healthy Volunteers 

Table 7.  Table 3 (continued). Studies of Healthy Volunteers 

Table 7.  Table 3(continued). Studies of Healthy Volunteers 
Table 7.  Table 3(continued). Studies of Healthy Volunteers 

Critiques and Conclusions 

Concentration-Effect Relations. The concentrations of anesthetics that block explicit memory are less than those that prevent voluntary responses to verbal commands. [17–20,51] Thus anesthetic concentrations that prevent voluntary responses also prevent conscious memory. At minimum alveolar concentration-awake and Cp50-awake (the plasma concentration required to prevent responsiveness to command in 50% of patients), both explicit and implicit memory are abolished. These results are inconsistent with reports of implicit memory during general anesthesia.

Although Block et al. [107–109] found that 30% nitrous oxide impaired performance on direct tests of memory but not some indirect tests of memory, Chortkoff et al. [82] found that implicit learning was suppressed at concentrations of isoflurane nearly identical to those that suppressed explicit learning. The inconsistency extends even to explicit memory. Contrary to several reports, [107–109,121–124] Dwyer et al. [51] found no impairment with a 30% nitrous oxide concentration. Newton et al. [110] found that recall and recognition of neutral words were lost at 0.2 minimum alveolar concentration of isoflurane inhalation. In contrast, Chortkoff et al. [82] found that this concentration was only the median effective dose for explicit learning, and the 95% effective dose was 0.4 minimum alveolar concentration. Zacny et al. [116] found that immediate and delayed recall were still possible during and after inhalation of 0.6% isoflurane. It is probable that the inconsistencies of these results are caused by methodologic flaws, some of which we describe.

Electroencephalographic Changes. Neither Munglani et al., [114] Andrade et al., [115] nor Dwyer et al. [112] provided convincing evidence of correlations between auditory evoked response (AER) or other electroencephalographic parameters and responsiveness to commands or memory of information presented during anesthetic administration. Future work might identify some parameter that unambiguously defines the point of loss of consciousness.

Methodologic Issues. There are two components of value in the design of experiments investigating the effects of drugs on cognition. The first is comparison of the behavior of the patient before and after administration of the drug, allowing unambiguous attribution of behavioral changes to the influence of the drug in question. The second design component is the use of a nondrug (placebo) control in which the same or other persons receive identical treatment except for administration of the drug. Pre-post comparisons alone are imperfect because practice on experimental tasks (Figure 2), environmental influences, fatigue, and other factors (Figure 3) can change behavior over time and affect the comparison of performance before and after drug administration. Comparison of separate treatment and control groups alone does not provide evidence that the groups were equivalent before treatment, so the possibilities that observed differences could have been present regardless of treatment or true differences could have been masked by different baseline levels between groups cannot be excluded. Although some studies with subanesthetic concentrations conducted since our previous review have shown some methodologic improvements relative to earlier work, many have omitted one or the other of these design components.

Figure 2. The mean number of digits recalled at intervals before and after diazepam and placebo treatments. Zero was the time of drug administration. Different random 15-digit numbers were presented each time. The score represents the mean of three trials. A delayed practice effect caused by varying doses of diazepam is apparent. (Reproduced with permission from Ghoneim et al. [151])

Figure 2. The mean number of digits recalled at intervals before and after diazepam and placebo treatments. Zero was the time of drug administration. Different random 15-digit numbers were presented each time. The score represents the mean of three trials. A delayed practice effect caused by varying doses of diazepam is apparent. (Reproduced with permission from Ghoneim et al. [151])

Close modal

Figure 3. Composite mood evaluation scores on visual analog scales before and after administration of diazepam and placebo. The vertical axis represents the mean value of seven scales. A decreasing score is associated with increasing sedation. The figure illustrates changes in subjective mood evaluations over time with or without active drug treatment. (Reproduced with permission from Ghoneim et al. [152]).

Figure 3. Composite mood evaluation scores on visual analog scales before and after administration of diazepam and placebo. The vertical axis represents the mean value of seven scales. A decreasing score is associated with increasing sedation. The figure illustrates changes in subjective mood evaluations over time with or without active drug treatment. (Reproduced with permission from Ghoneim et al. [152]).

Close modal

Some investigators, rather than testing different concentrations of an anesthetic in separate sessions or with separate participants, have used a series of increasing and then decreasing concentrations of the drug within a single session. Practice effects, fatigue effects, cumulative effects of the anesthetic agent, tolerance development, proactive or retroactive interference, and other potential confounders are inherent risks of this type of design. Some investigators administered the indirect test(s) of memory only during the inhalation of the highest concentration of the anesthetic (e.g., Chortkoff et al. [82] and Munglani et al. [114]). In the absence of testing in a control group, during inhalation of lower concentrations of the anesthetic, or both, it is difficult to determine whether the absence of learning was due to the effect of the drug, insensitivity of the task, or both. Other concerns are use of different lists of words with no evidence that they were comparable in their normative characteristics or had been equated in pilot studies; absence of counterbalancing of lists over treatments, which may confound differences among lists with differences among treatments; and use of tasks that have not been adequately validated.

Brain Imaging. Studies of the effects of anesthetics on memory may eventually take advantage of the remarkable advances in noninvasive functional brain-imaging research. Correlates of anatomic systems involved in perception, memory, and other mental functions in the brains of living persons are being studied using positron emission tomography combined with magnetic resonance imaging. These technologies can sample anywhere within the brain, measuring changes in blood flow that occur in conjunction with neuronal activity. Measurements are made while participants perform specified behavioral tasks. By imaging blood flow multiple times during performance of different tasks, the experimenter can relate specific functional attributes with activity in local cortical areas. Learning with and without awareness may involve different anatomic systems. Working memory, explicit memory, and implicit memory involve activity that is highly distributed in multiple cortical, subcortical, and cerebellar areas. Priming is associated with decrements in activity in auditory or visual association cortex, depending on the modality of testing. Other types of implicit memory use different anatomic systems. [125]

Electroencephalography records electric activity of the brain from the scalp and hence does not accurately localize the source of neuronal activity. It records the signals associated with neuronal activity over a much shorter time period than positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. A combination of brain imaging and electric recording may complement each other and define the anatomy of the circuits and the time course of events in these circuits that are involved in a particular behavior. [126] Studies of healthy volunteers in the awake state and during anesthesia using well-defined cognitive tasks may provide answers to questions difficult to obtain in other ways.

Studies in anesthetized patients having surgery who were subjected to multitudes of extraneous influences, which constitute most of the current literature, may not produce conclusive and convincing results for implicit memory during anesthesia. Animals offer the opportunity to gather information that is not obtainable in any other way and allow tighter control of the experimental procedures.

Methods of Studying Learning and Memory and the Effects of Anesthetics 

Classical conditioning and operant conditioning probably are responsible for most of an animal's learned responses. [127] Compared with humans, animals often require elaborate training programs and possess a more restricted repertoire of behavior. It is difficult to dissociate implicit and explicit memory systems in animals and determine whether a particular task taps into one of these systems or the other. The classical or Pavlovian conditioning paradigm includes several different procedures that can be used to study various aspects of the conditioned response. All of these procedures have in common the association of a neutral conditioned stimulus, such as a tone or white noise, with an unconditioned stimulus, such as an electric shock or air puff. Initially, the conditioned stimulus does not elicit a response but the unconditioned stimulus elicits an unconditioned response. With repeated pairings, however, the conditioned stimulus alone also elicits a response-the conditioned response (Figure 4). Food aversions can be conditioned with very long interstimulus intervals of several hours between conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus. When an animal consumes a flavored fluid and becomes nauseated afterward, it will acquire a conditioned flavor aversion associating the flavor with sickness.

Figure 4. The events of a classical or Pavlovian conditioning trial before a conditioned response (CR) is established and after. The upward deflection of the trace indicates stimulus or response onset. The downward deflection indicates offset. A tone (the CS or conditioned stimulus) is paired with shock (the US or unconditioned stimulus). The latter elicits the unconditioned response (UCR). Responses may be salivation, eyelid closure, increased skin conductance, and so on. During later conditioning trials, repeated pairings of the tone and shock elicits a response to the tone (the CR) and to the shock.

Figure 4. The events of a classical or Pavlovian conditioning trial before a conditioned response (CR) is established and after. The upward deflection of the trace indicates stimulus or response onset. The downward deflection indicates offset. A tone (the CS or conditioned stimulus) is paired with shock (the US or unconditioned stimulus). The latter elicits the unconditioned response (UCR). Responses may be salivation, eyelid closure, increased skin conductance, and so on. During later conditioning trials, repeated pairings of the tone and shock elicits a response to the tone (the CR) and to the shock.

Close modal

Current Literature 

The literature on the effects of anesthetics on learning and memory in animals is limited (Table 4). The results of the studies cover a wide spectrum, including findings of no learning during anesthesia, learning only when epinephrine is administered, learning during ketamine anesthesia in which there is sympathetic nerve stimulation, and learning without epinephrine or apparent sympathetic nerve stimulation (Figure 5). Conditioned flavor aversion was demonstrated when lithium was administered during anesthesia.

Table 4. Studies of Animal Learning and Memory During Anesthesia 

Table 4. Studies of Animal Learning and Memory During Anesthesia 
Table 4. Studies of Animal Learning and Memory During Anesthesia 

Figure 5. Conditioned fear was measured by suppression of drinking in mice 24 h after training and recovery from anesthesia. The figure shows mean suppression ratios +/- SEM; that is, the duration of drinking in the presence of a tone-conditioned stimulus (CS), divided by duration of drinking in the absence of the tone. Lower values reflect greater conditioning. Four groups of animals were anesthetized. One group (CS-UCS group at left) was conditioned during anesthesia by pairing of CS with an unconditioned stimulus (UCS), an electric shock. The other three anesthetized groups (CS Only, UCS Only, and CS-UCS Delayed) were controls and were not conditioned. An additional group in which the animals were conditioned in the absence of anesthesia (CS-UCS group at right) was included to evaluate the strength of conditioning in unanesthetized animals. The group that was conditioned during anesthesia (CS-UCS group at left) showed significant conditioning as assessed by the suppression ratio compared with the three anesthetized control groups. (Reproduced with permission from Pang et al. [135]).

Figure 5. Conditioned fear was measured by suppression of drinking in mice 24 h after training and recovery from anesthesia. The figure shows mean suppression ratios +/- SEM; that is, the duration of drinking in the presence of a tone-conditioned stimulus (CS), divided by duration of drinking in the absence of the tone. Lower values reflect greater conditioning. Four groups of animals were anesthetized. One group (CS-UCS group at left) was conditioned during anesthesia by pairing of CS with an unconditioned stimulus (UCS), an electric shock. The other three anesthetized groups (CS Only, UCS Only, and CS-UCS Delayed) were controls and were not conditioned. An additional group in which the animals were conditioned in the absence of anesthesia (CS-UCS group at right) was included to evaluate the strength of conditioning in unanesthetized animals. The group that was conditioned during anesthesia (CS-UCS group at left) showed significant conditioning as assessed by the suppression ratio compared with the three anesthetized control groups. (Reproduced with permission from Pang et al. [135]).

Close modal

Critiques and Conclusions 

The development of flavor aversions when the nauseating agent is administered during anesthesia can provide evidence for learning during anesthesia. However, none of the available reports [131,137–140] provide a compelling result. The depth and duration of anesthesia produced by bolus doses of pentobarbital or urethane are difficult to determine, as are the durations of actions of nauseating agents such as lithium chloride or apomorphine. It is possible that the effects of the latter may linger during recovery from anesthesia.

Classical conditioning may be the best method available to investigate learning and memory during anesthesia in animals. Fear conditioning has been more successful in providing evidence of learning during anesthesia than conditioning of a skeletal muscle response, such as eye-blink conditioning. The application of a strong, painful, potentially life-threatening and anesthesia-lightening unconditioned stimulus compared with a slight electrotactile one might account for the different results. However, a recent study [136] that used conditioned fear reported negative findings. The question of the possibility of learning during “adequate” anesthesia in animals has yet to be answered.

Wakefulness during anesthesia without postoperative explicit recall may occur during light anesthesia and is confirmed by studies using the isolated forearm technique. [17–20,141] The amnesia is due to the effects of anesthetics on acquisition, retrieval, or both. Blockade of retrieval may also be due to the traumatic nature of information that is acquired during surgery, state-dependency (state-dependent retrieval refers to the improved memory when recall and learning occur in the same context), or both. [142,143] Are episodes of intraoperative consciousness without subsequent explicit recall harmful? It is possible that if memories of intraoperative events cannot be consciously recollected, they may exert a stronger influence on emotions than in the normal waking state for two reasons. First, anesthetized patients are less able to defend themselves against the implications of what they have “heard”; that is, to use normal, conscious cognitive processes to rationalize or cope with “bad news.” Second, information that bypasses consciousness may activate complexes (repressed systems of emotionally charged ideas), with unfortunate psychological consequences, such as postoperative anxiety or depression. [144–147]

The evidence for negative effects resulting from unfavorable comments voiced during anesthesia remains circumstantial. [15,21,148] Case reports, however, cannot serve to establish a phenomenon or substitute for controlled studies, particularly in cases in which suggestive techniques such as hypnosis were used. Unfortunately, controlled studies, for ethical and legal reasons, use neutral stimuli. Some investigators [15,149–150] claim that meaningfulness of the material to be learned is of paramount importance, and it is difficult in controlled studies to use personally meaningful or emotional materials comparable to those mentioned in some case reports. Therein lies a dilemma. Case reports cannot establish a cause-and-effect relation between unconscious learning during anesthesia and a patient's psychological disorder or determine the frequency of such occurrences. But neither should they be dismissed, particularly if there is corroborating evidence from persons other than the patient and the therapist concerning the remembered intraoperative events. For several years, there have been no new reports linking unconscious learning during anesthesia and postoperative psychosomatic disorders.

We have summarized the recent literature in the context of earlier studies. We have also highlighted some of the pitfalls and shortcomings and in some cases provided suggestions for future studies. Several conclusions can be drawn:

1. Conscious recall of intraoperative events is rare. It appears to be a dose-related phenomenon and is at highest risk during concomitant use of muscle relaxants. The epidemiologic nature of and possible measures to prevent post-traumatic stress disorder, which sometimes follows, must be studied. The need to develop an “awareness monitor” and the justification for its cost are controversial.

2. Studies of anesthetized patients using implicit tests of memory continue to generate positive and negative results. Studies using auditory evoked responses or other methods to measure anesthetic depth or using an experimental manipulation that is thought to produce one effect on explicit tasks and another effect on implicit tasks may elucidate the nature of memory for events during anesthesia. The influence of experimental variables that determine the positive outcome of studies need to be explored. Replication of successful results is important and should be attempted within the framework of new studies. For example, if investigators want to determine whether patients have unconscious memory for material presented while the patient was anesthetized with a particular anesthetic regimen, they should study another group of patients anesthetized with another regimen, which, in a previous study using the same memory task(s), showed evidence of unconscious memory.

We can speculate that unconscious memory occurs only in few patients, only some of the time, and during light levels of anesthesia. Learning may be more perceptual than engaging in elaborate processing of complex information and may be limited to single, relatively familiar words. Memory may be more evident if tested as soon as possible after surgery.

3. Some recent studies of the efficacy of administration of therapeutic suggestions during anesthesia have reported positive results. However, methodologic and statistical concerns plague some of this work.

4. A concentration of 0.4% end-expired isoflurane prevents both recall and responses to commands in volunteers; 60% nitrous oxide does not completely prevent explicit memory or responses to commands. The effect of surgical stimulation and salience of the stimuli in raising the concentrations that prevent memory is unknown. Neither auditory-evoked responses nor bispectral analyses of the electroencephalograph have yet provided clear evidence of concentrations at which consciousness ceases. Use of brain imaging techniques may provide important information.

5. Animal studies, which may allow tight control of experimental procedures, have nevertheless produced contradictory results concerning learning and memory during anesthesia. Fear conditioning, together with precise control of the anesthetic concentrations, may be the most promising method.

6. It has been reported that psychic trauma may result from the unconscious storage of traumatic events experienced during anesthesia. However, for this hypothesis to be generally accepted, we need more evidence than anecdotal case reports.

7. Interest in the subject of learning and memory during anesthesia continues to be strong and shows no sign of abating. A dedicated group of anesthesiologists, psychologists, and others are working to unravel some of the mysteries surrounding this subject.

*Domino KB: Closed malpractice claims for awareness during anesthesia. ASA Newsletter 1996; 60:14–17.

**Bennett HL: Treating psychological sequelae of awareness. American Society of Anesthesiologists Newsletter 1994; 58:12–15.

1.
Ghoneim MM, Block RI: Learning and consciousness during general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1992; 76:279-305.
2.
Kihlstrom JF: Implicit memory function during anesthesia, Memory and Awareness in Anesthesia. Edited by Sebel PS, Bonke B, Winograd E. Englewood Cliffs, Prentice Hall, 1993, pp 10-30.
3.
Sebel PS, Bonke B, Winograd E: Preface, Memory and Awareness in Anesthesia. Englewood Cliffs, Prentice-Hall, 1993, pp 6-7.
4.
Schacter DL: Priming and multiple memory systems: perceptual mechanisms of implicit memory. J Cognitive Neuroscience 1992; 4:244-56.
5.
Squire LR: Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. J Cognitive Neuroscience 1992; 4:232-43.
6.
Liu WHD, Thorp TAS, Graham SG, Aitkenhead AR: Incidence of awareness with recall during general anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 1991; 46:435-7.
7.
Sandin R, Nordstrom O: Awareness during total i.v. anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1993; 71:782-7.
8.
Phillips AA, McLean RF, Devitt JH, Harrington EM: Recall of intraoperative events after general anaesthesia and cardiopulmonary bypass. Can J Anaesth 1993; 40:922-6.
9.
Ranta S, Jussila J, Hynynen M: Recall of awareness during cardiac anaesthesia: influence of feedback information to the anaesthesiologist. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1996; 40:554-60.
10.
Lyons G, Macdonald R: Awareness during caesarean section. Anaesthesia 1991; 46:62-4.
11.
Bogetz MS, Katz JA: Recall of surgery for major trauma. Anesthesiology 1984; 61:6-9.
12.
Jones JG: Perception and memory during general anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1994; 73:31-7.
13.
Cobcroft MD, Forsdick C: Awareness under anaesthesia: The patient's point of view. Anaesth Intensive Care 1993; 21:837-43.
14.
Moerman N, Bonke B, Oosting J: Awareness and recall during general anesthesia. Facts and feelings. Anesthesiology 1993; 79:454-64.
15.
Blacher RS: The psychological experience of surgery. New York, Wiley, 1987, pp 1-25.
16.
Glass P, Gan TJ, Sebel PS, Rosow C, Kearse L, Bloom M, Manberg P: Comparison of the bispectral index (BIS) and measured drug concentrations for the monitoring effects of propofol, midazolam, alfentanil and isoflurane. Anesthesiology 1995; 83:A374.
17.
Russell IF: Comparison of wakefulness with two anaesthetic regimens: total iv v. balanced anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1986; 58:965-8.
18.
Russell IF: Conscious awareness during general anaesthesia: Relevance of autonomic signs and isolated arm movements as guides to depth of anesthesia, Depth of Anaesthesia. Clinical Anaesthesiology, Volume 3. Edited by Jones JG. London, Bailliere Tindall, 1989, pp 511-32.
19.
Russell IF: Midazolam-alfentanil: An anaesthetic? An investigation using the isolated forearm technique. Br J Anaesth 1993; 70:42-6.
20.
King H-K, Ashley S, Brathwaite D, Decayette J, Wooten DJ: Adequacy of general anesthesia for cesarean section. Anesth Analg 1993; 77:84-8.
21.
Howard JF: Incidents of auditory perception during general anesthesia with traumatic sequelae. Med J Aust 1987; 146:44-6.
22.
Aitkenhead AR: Conscious awareness, Memory and Awareness in Anesthesia. Edited by Sebel PS, Bonke B, Winograd E. Englewood Cliffs, Prentice-Hall, 1993, pp 386-99.
23.
Dexter F, Coffin S, Tinker JH: Decreases in anesthesia-controlled time cannot permit one additional surgical operation to be reliably scheduled during the workday. Anesth Analg 1995; 81:1263-8.
24.
Dexter F, Tinker JH: analysis of strategies to decrease postanesthesia care unit costs. Anesthesiology 1995; 82:94-101.
25.
Schacter DL: Implicit knowledge: new perspectives on unconscious processes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1992; 89:11113-17.
26.
Merikle PM, Reingold EM: Comparing direct (explicit) and indirect (implicit) measures to study unconscious memory. J Exp Psychol (Learn Mem Cogn) 1991; 17:224-33.
27.
Jacoby LL, Toth J, Yonelinas A: Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: Measuring recollection. J Exp Psychol (Gen) 1993; 122:139-54.
28.
Schacter DL, Bowers J, Booker J: Intention, awareness and implicit memory: The retrieval intentionality criterion, Implicit Memory: Theoretical Issues. Edited by Lewandowsky S, Dunn JC, Kirsner K. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum, 1989, pp 47-65.
29.
Roorda-Hrdlickova V, Wolters G, Bonke B, Phaf RH: Unconscious perception during general anaesthesia demonstrated by an implicit memory task, Memory and Awareness in Anaesthesia. Edited by Bonke B, Fitch W, Millar K. Amsterdam, Swets & Zeitlinger, 1990, pp 150-5.
30.
Jelicic M, Bonke B, Appelboom DK: Indirect memory for words presented during anaesthesia. Lancet 1990; 2:249.
31.
Block RI, Ghoneim MM, SumPing ST, Ali MA: Human learning during general anaesthesia and surgery. Br J Anaesth 1991; 66:170-8.
32.
Brown AS, Best MR, Mitchell DB, Haggard LC: Memory under anesthesia: Evidence for response suppression. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1992; 30:244-6.
33.
Westmoreland CL, Sebel PS, Winograd E, Goldman WP: Indirect memory during anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1993; 78:237-41.
34.
Bonebakker AE, Bonke B, Klein J, Wolters G, Hop WCJ: Implicit memory during balanced anaesthesia: lack of evidence. Anaesthesia 1993; 48:657-60.
35.
Ghoneim MM, Block RI, Fowles DC: No evidence of classical conditioning of electrodermal responses during anaesthesia. Anesthesiology 1992; 76:682-8.
36.
Jelicic M, De Roode A, Bovill JG, Bonke B: Unconscious learning during anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 1992; 47:835-7.
37.
Jelicic M, Asbury AJ, Millar K, Bonke B: Implicit learning during enflurane anaesthesia in spontaneously breathing patients? Anaesthesia 1993; 48:766-8.
38.
DeRoode A, Jelicic M, Bonke B, Bovill JG: The effect of midazolam premedication on implicit memory activation during alfentanil-nitrous oxide anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 1995; 50:191-4.
39.
Donker AG, Phaf RH, Porcelijn T, Bonke B: Processing familiar and unfamiliar auditory stimuli during general anesthesia. Anesth Analg 1996; 82:452-5.
40.
Lewis SA, Jenkinson J, Wilson J: An EEG investigation of awareness during anaesthesia. Br J Psychol 1973; 64:413-5.
41.
Kihlstrom JF, Schacter DL, Cork RC, Hurt CA, Behr SE: Implicit and explicit memory following surgical anesthesia. Psychol Sci 1990; 1:303-6.
42.
Cork RC, Kihlstrom JF, Schacter DL: Absence of explicit and implicit memory in patients anesthetized with sufentanil/nitrous oxide. Anesthesiology 1992; 76:892-8.
43.
Bethune DW, Ghosh S, Gray L, Kerr L, Walker IA, Doolan LA, Harwood RJ, Sharples LD: Learning during general anaesthesia: Implicit recall after methohexitone or propfol infusion. Br J Anaesth 1992; 69:197-9.
44.
Schwender D, Kaiser A, Klasing S, Peter K, Poppel E: Midlatency auditory evoked potentials and explicit and implicit memory in patients undergoing cardiac surgery. Anesthesiology 1994; 80:493-501.
45.
Dubovsky SL, Trustman R: Absence of recall after general anesthesia. Implications for theory and practice. Anesth Analg 1976; 55:696-701.
46.
Millar K, Watkinson N: Recognition of words presented during general anaesthesia. Ergonomics 1983; 26:585-94.
47.
Eich E, Reeves JL, Katz RL: Anesthesia, amnesia, and the memory/awareness distinction. Anesth Anal 1985; 64:1143-8.
48.
Evans C, Richardson PH: Improved recovery and reduced postoperative stay after therapeutic suggestions during general anaesthesia. Lancet 1988; 2:491-3.
49.
Caseley-Rondi G, Merikle PM, Bowers KS: Unconscious cognition in the context of general anesthesia. Conscious Cog 1994; 3:166-95.
50.
Goldmann L: Information-processing under general anaesthesia-a review. J Roy Soc Med 1988; 81:224-7.
51.
Dwyer R, Bennett HL, Eger EI II, Heilbron D: Effects of isoflurane and nitrous oxide in subanesthetic concentrations on memory and responsiveness in volunteers. Anesthesiology 1992; 77:888-98.
52.
Winograd E, Sebel PS, Goldman WP, Clifton CL, Lowden JD: Indirect assessment of memory for music during anaesthesia. J Clin Anesth 1991; 3:276-9.
53.
Bonke B, Van Dam ME, VanKleef JW, Slijper FME: Implicit memory tested in children during inhalation anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 1992; 47:747-9.
54.
Kalff AC, Bonke B, Wolters G, Manger FW: Implicit memory for stimuli presented during inhalation anesthesia in children. Psychological Reports 1995; 77:371-5.
55.
Bonebakker AE, Bonke B, Klein J, Wolters G, Stijnen T, Passchier J, Merikle PM: Information-processing during balanced anaesthesia: evidence for unconscious memory. Memory and Cognition 1996; 24:766-76.
56.
Mandler G: Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. Psychol Rev 1980; 87:252-71.
57.
Merikle PM: Perception without awareness: Critical issues. Am Psychol 1992; 47:792-5.
58.
Merikle PM: On the futility of attempting to demonstrate null awareness. Behav Brain Sci 1994; 17:412.
59.
Jacoby LL, Woloshyn V, Kelley C: Becoming famous without being recognized: unconscious influences of memory produced by dividing attention. J Exp Psychol (Gen) 1989; 118:115-25.
60.
Ghoneim MM, Mewaldt SP: Benzodiazepines and human memory: A review. Anesthesiology 1990; 72:926-38.
61.
Ghoneim MM, Block RI, Sum Ping ST, E-Zahaby HM, Hinrichs JV: The interactions of midazolam and flumazenil on human memory and cognition. Anesthesiology 1993; 79:1183-92.
62.
Brown MW, Brown J, Bowes JB: Absence of priming coupled with substantially preserved recognition in lorazepam-induced amnesia. Q J Exp Psychol 1989; 41A:599-617.
63.
Jelicic M, Bonke B: Implicit memory for stimuli presented during anaesthesia: Role of anaesthetic cocktail and memory test. Medical Hypotheses 1993; 41:353-4.
64.
Tulving E, Schacter DL: Priming and human memory systems. Science 1990; 247:301-6.
65.
Carlesimo GA: Perceptual and conceptual priming in amnesic and alcoholic patients. Neuropsychologia 1994; 32:903-21.
66.
Ghoneim MM, Block RI: Learning during anesthesia. Int Anesthesiol Clin 1993; 31:53-65.
67.
Schacter DL: Implicit memory: History and current status. J Exp Psychol (Learn Mem Cogn) 1987; 134:501-18.
68.
Kihlstrom JF, Schacter DL: Anaesthesia, amnesia, and the cognitive unconscious, Memory and Awareness in Anesthesia. Edited by Bonke B, Fitch W, Millar K. Amsterdam, Swets & Zeitlinger, 1990, pp 21-44.
69.
Foster KI, Davies C: Repetition priming and frequency attention in lexical access. J Exp Psychol (Learn Mem Cogn) 1984; 10:680-98.
70.
McLeod CM: Word context during initial exposure influences degree of priming in word-fragment completion. J Exp Psychol (Learn Mem Cogn) 1989; 15:398-406.
71.
Challis BH, Sidhu R: Dissociative effect of massed repetition on implicit and explicit measures of memory. J Exp Psychol (Learn Mem Cogn) 1993; 19:115-27.
72.
Musen G: Effects of verbal labeling and exposure duration on implicit memory for visual patterns. J Exp Psychol (Learn Mem Cogn) 1991; 17:954-62.
73.
Andrade J: Learning during anaesthesia: A review. Br J Psychol 1995; 86:479-506.
74.
Merikle PM, Daneman M: Memory for events during anesthesia: a meta-analysis, Memory and Awareness in Anaesthesia III. Edited by Bonke B, Bovill JG, Moerman N. Assen, The Netherlands, Van Gorcum, 1996, pp 108-21.
75.
Karni A, Tanne D, Rubenstein BS, Askenasy JJM, Sagi D: Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. Science 1994; 265:679-82.
76.
Rosenberg J, Wildschiosdtz G, Pedersen MH, von Jessen F, Kehlet H: Late postoperative nocturnal episodic hypoxaemia and associated sleep pattern. Br J Anaesth 1994; 72:145-50.
77.
Millar K: Efficacy of therapeutic suggestions presented during anaesthesia: Re-analysis of conflicting results. Br J Anaesth 1993; 71:597-601.
78.
Bennett HL, Davis HS, Giannini JA: Nonverbal response to intraoperative conversation. Br J Anaest 1985; 57:174-9.
79.
Goldmann L, Shah MV, Hebden MW: Memory of cardiac anaesthesia: Psychological sequelae in cardiac patients of intraoperative suggestion and operating room conversation. Anaesthesia 1987; 42:596-603.
80.
Dwyer R, Bennett HL, Eger EI II, Peterson N: Isoflurane anesthesia prevents unconscious learning. Anesth Analg 1992; 75:107-12.
81.
Jansen CK, Bonke B, Klein J, van Dasselaar N, Hop WCJ: Failure to demonstrate unconscious perception during balanced anaesthesia by postoperative motor response. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1991; 35:407-10.
82.
Chortkoff BS, Bennett HL, Eger EI II: Subanesthetic concentrations of isoflurane suppress learning as defined by the category-example task. Anesthesiology 1993; 79:16-22.
83.
Gonsowski CT, Chortkoff BS, Eger EI II, Bennett HL, Weiskopf RB: Subanesthetic concentrations of desflurane and isoflurane suppress explicit and implicit learning. Anesth Analg 1995; 80:568-72.
84.
Wolfe LS, Millet JB: Control of post-operative pain by suggestion under general anesthesia. Am J Clin Hypn 1960; 3:109-12.
85.
Hutchings DD: The value of suggestion given under anesthesia: a report and evaluation of 200 consecutive cases. Am J Clin Hypn 1961; 4:16-9.
86.
Pearson RE: Response to suggestions given under general anesthesia. Am J Clin Hypn 1961; 4:106-14.
87.
Abramson M, Greenfield I, Heron WT: Response to or perception of auditory stimuli under deep surgical anesthesia. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1966; 96:584-5.
88.
Bonke B, Schmitz PIM, Verhage F, Zwaveling A: Clinical study of so-called unconscious perception during general anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1986; 58:957-64.
89.
Woo R, Seltzer JL, Marr A: The lack of response to suggestion under controlled surgical anaesthesia. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1987; 31:567-71.
90.
Boeke S, Bonke B, Bouwhuis-Hoogerwerf ML, Bovill JG, Zwaveling A: Effects of sounds presented during general anaesthesia on postoperative course. Br J Anaesth 1988; 60:697-702.
91.
McLintock TTC, Aitken H, Downie CFA, Kenny GNC: Postoperative analgesic requirements in patients exposed to positive intraoperative suggestions. BMJ 1990; 301:788-90.
92.
Block RI, Ghoneim MM, Sum Ping ST, Ali MA: Efficacy of therapeutic suggestions for improved postoperative recovery presented during general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1991; 75:746-55.
93.
Liu WHD, Standen PJ, Aitkenhead AR: Therapeutic suggestions during general anaesthesia in patients undergoing hysterectomy. Br J Anaesth 1992; 68:277-81.
94.
Korunka C, Guttmann G, Schleinitz D, Hilpert M, Haas R, Ritzal S: Die auswirkung von suggestionen und musik wahren vollnarkose auf postoperative befindlichkeit (The effect of suggestions and music during general anesthesia on postoperative well-being). Zeitschrift fur Klinische Psychologie 1992; 21:272-85.
95.
Jelicic M, Bonke B, Millar K: Effect of different therapeutic suggestions presented during anaesthesia on post-operative course. Eur J Anaesthesiol 1993; 10:343-7.
96.
Williams AR, Hind M, Sweeney BP: The incidence and severity of postoperative nausea and vomiting in patients exposed to positive intra-operative suggestions. Anaesthesia 1994; 49:340-2.
97.
Hughes JA, Sanders LD, Dunne JA, Tarpey J, Vickers MD: Reducing smoking: the effect of suggestion during general anaesthesia on postoperative smoking habits. Anaesthesia 1994; 49:126-8.
98.
Oddby-Muhrbeck E, Jakobsson J, Enquist B: Implicit processing and therapeutic suggestion during balanced anesthesia. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 1995; 39:333-7.
99.
Van der Laan WH, van Leeuwen BL, Sebel PS, Winograd E, Baumann PL, Bonke B: Therapeutic suggestion has no effect on postoperative morphine requirements. Anesth Analg 1996; 82:148-52.
100.
Myles PS, Hendrata M, Layher Y, Williams NJ, Hall JL, Moloney JT, Powell J: Double-blind, randomized trial of cessation of smoking after audiotape suggestion during anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1996; 76:694-8.
101.
Andrade J, Munglani R: Therapeutic suggestions during general anaesthesia [Letter]. Br J Anaesth 1994; 72:730.
102.
Richmond CE, Bromley LM, Woolf CJ: Preoperative morphine pre-empts postoperative pain. Lancet 1993; 342:73-5.
103.
Mansfield MD, James KS, Kinsella J: Influence of dose and timing of administration of morphine on postoperative pain and analgesic requirements. Br J Anaesth 1996; 76:358-61.
104.
d'Amours RH, Kramer TH, Mannes AJ: A Monte Carlo simulation of iv PCA use to examine its sensitivity as an analgesic assay. Anesthesiology 1995; 83:A790.
105.
Muir JJ, Warner MN, Offord KP, Buck CF, Harper JV, Kunkel SE: Role of nitrous oxide and other factors in postoperative nausea and vomiting: A randomized and blinded prospective study. Anesthesiology 1987; 66:513-8.
106.
Anonymous: Nausea and vomiting after general anaesthesia [Editorial]. Lancet 1989; 1:651-2.
107.
Block RI, Ghoneim MM, Pathak D, Kumar V, Hinrichs JV: Effects of a subanesthetic concentration of nitrous oxide on overt and covert assessments of memory and associative processes. Psychopharmacology 1988; 96:324-31.
108.
Block RI, Ghoneim MM, Hinrichs JV, Kumar V, Pathak D: Effects of a subanesthetic concentration of nitrous oxide on memory and subjective experience. Influence of assessment procedures and types of stimuli. Hum Psychopharmacol 1988; 3:257-65.
109.
Block RI, Ghoneim MM, Fowles DC, Kumar V, Pathak D: Effects of a subanesthetic concentration of nitrous oxide on establishment, elicitation, and semantic and phonemic generalization of classically conditioned skin conductance responses. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1987; 28:7-14.
110.
Newton DEF, Thornton C, Konieczko KM, Frith CD, Dore CJ, Webster NR, Luff NP: Levels of consciousness in volunteers breathing sub-MAC concentrations of isoflurane. Br J Anaesth 1990; 65:609-15.
111.
Newton DEF, Thornton C, Konieczko KM, Jordan C, Webster NR, Luff NP, Frith CD, Dore DJ: Auditory evoked response and awareness: A study in volunteers at sub-MAC concentrations of isoflurane. Br J Anaesth 1992; 69:122-9.
112.
Dwyer RC, Rampil IJ, Eger EI II, Bennett HL: The electroencephalogram does not predict depth of isoflurane anesthesia. Anesthesiology 1994; 81:403-9.
113.
Chortkoff BS, Bennett HL, Eger EI II: Does nitrous oxide antagonize isoflurane-induced suppression of learning? Anesthesiology 1993; 79:724-32.
114.
Munglani R, Andrade J, Sapsford DJ, Baddeley A, Jones JG: A measure of consciousness and memory during isoflurane administration: the coherent frequency. Br J Anaesth 1993; 71:633-41.
115.
Andrade J, Munglani R, Jones JG, Baddeley AD: Cognitive performance during anesthesia. Conscious Cog 1994; 3:148-65.
116.
Zacny JP, Sparacino G, Hoffmann PM, Martin R, Lichtor JL: The subjective, behavioral and cognitive effects of subanesthetic concentrations of isoflurane and nitrous oxide in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology 1994; 114:409-16.
117.
Chortkoff BS, Eger EI II, Crankshaw DP, Gonsowski CT, Dutton RC, Ionescu P: Concentrations of desflurane and propofol that suppress response to command in humans. Anesth Analg 1995; 81:737-43.
118.
Chortkoff BS, Gonsowski CT, Bennett HL, Levinson B, Crankshaw DP, Dutton RC, Ionescu P, Block RI, Eger EI II: Subanesthetic concentrations of desflurane and propofol suppress recall of emotionally charged information. Anesth Analg 1995; 81:728-36.
119.
Zacny JP, Yajnik S, Lichtor JL, Klafta JM, Young CJ, Thapar P, Rupani G, Carlson DW, Apfelbaum JL: The acute and residual effects of subanesthetic concentrations of isoflurane/nitrous oxide combinations on cognitive and psychomotor performance in healthy volunteers. Anesth Analg 1996; 82:153-7.
120.
Gonsowski CT, Chortkoff BS, Eger EI II, Bennett HL, Weiskopf RB: Subanesthetic concentrations of desflurane and isoflurane suppress explicit and implicit learning. Anesth Analg 1995; 80:568-72.
121.
Mewaldt SP, Ghoneim MM, Choi WW, Korttila K, Petersen RC: Nitrous oxide and human state-dependent memory. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1988; 30:83-7.
122.
Ghoneim MM, Mewaldt SP, Petersen RC: Subanesthetic concentration of nitrous oxide and human memory. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1981; 5:395-402.
123.
Korttila K, Ghoneim MM, Jacobs L, Mewaldt SP, Petersen RC: Time course of mental and psychomotor effects of 30 percent nitrous oxide during inhalation and recovery. Anesthesiology 1981; 54:220-6.
124.
Parkhouse J, Henrie JR, Duncan GM, Rome HP: Nitrous oxide analgesia in relation to mental performance. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1960; 128:44-54.
125.
Grafton ST: Mapping memory systems in the human brain. Semin Neurosci 1995; 7:157-63.
126.
Raichle ME: Images of the human mind. Neuropsychopharmacology 1994; 10:28S-33S.
127.
Mackintosh NJ: The psychology of animal learning. New York, Academic Press, 1974.
128.
Weinberger NM, Gold PE, Sternberg DB: Epinephrine enables Pavlovian fear conditioning under anesthesia. Science 1984; 233:605-7.
129.
Gold PE, Weinberger NM, Sternberg DB: Epinephrine-induced learning under anesthesia: Retention performance at several training-testing intervals. Behav Neurosci 1985; 99:1019-22.
130.
Edeline J-M, Massioui N: Retention of CS-US association learned under ketamine anesthesia. Brain Res 1988; 457:274-80.
131.
Bermudez-Rattoni F, Forthman DL, Sanchez MA, Perez JL, Garcia J: Odor and taste aversions conditioned in anesthetized rats. Behav Neurosci 1988; 102:726-32.
132.
Dariola MK, Yadava A, Malhotra S: Effect of epinephrine on learning under anaesthesia. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology 1993; 19:47-51.
133.
Ghoneim MM, Chen P, El-Zahaby HM, Block RI: Ketamine: acquisition and retention of classically conditioned responses during treatment with large doses. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1994; 49:1061-6.
134.
El-Zahaby HM, Ghoneim MM, Johnson GM, Gormezano I: Effects of subanesthetic concentrations of isoflurane and their interactions with epinephrine on acquisition and retention of the rabbit nictitating membrane response. Anesthesiology 1994; 81:229-37.
135.
Pang R, Turndorf H, Quartermain D: Pavlovian fear conditioning in mice anesthetized with halothane. Physiol Behav 1996; 59:873-5.
136.
Kandel L, Chortkoff BS, Sonner J, Laster MJ, Eger EI II: Nonanesthetics can suppress learning. Anesth Analg 1996; 82:321-6.
137.
Roll DL, Smith JC: Conditioned taste aversion in anesthetized rats, Biological Boundaries of Learning. Edited by Seligman MEP, Hager JL. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972, pp 98-102.
138.
Rozin P, Ree P: Long extension of effective CS-US interval by anesthesia between CS and US. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1972; 80:43.
139.
Millner JR, Palfai T: Metrazol impairs conditioned aversion produced by Li Cl: a time dependent effect. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1975; 3:201-4.
140.
Buresova O, Bures J: The effect of anesthesia on acquisition and extinction of conditioned taste aversion. Behav Biol 1977; 20:41-50.
141.
Tunstall ME: Detecting wakefulness during general anaesthesia for caesarean section. BMJ 1977; 1:1321.
142.
Andrade J, Baddeley A: Human memory and anesthesia, Depth of Anesthesia. Edited by Jones JG. Int Anesthesiol Clin 1993; 31:39-51.
143.
Overton DA: Historical context of state dependent learning and discriminative drug effects. Behav Pharmacol 1991; 2:253-64.
144.
Dixon NF: Unconscious perception and general anaesthesia. Baillieres Clin Anesthesiol 1989; 3:473-85.
145.
LeDoux JE: Cognitive-emotional interactions in the brain. Cognition and Emotion 1989; 3:267-89.
146.
Bornstein RF: Subliminal mere exposure effects, Perception without Awareness: Cognitive, Clinical and Social Perspectives. Edited by Bornstein RF, Pittman TS. New York, Guilford Press, 1992, pp 191-210.
147.
Murphy ST, Zajonc RB: Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. J Pers Soc Psychol 1993; 65:723-39.
148.
Levinson BW: States of awareness during general anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 1965; 37:544-6.
149.
Levinson BW: Quo vadis, Memory and Awareness in Anesthesia. Edited by Sebel PS, Bonke B, Winograd E. Englewood Cliffs, Prentice-Hall, 1993, pp 498-500.
150.
Bennett HL: Memory for events during anesthesia does occur: A psychologist's viewpoint, Memory and Awareness in Anesthesia. Edited by Sebel PS, Bonke B, Winograd E. Englewood Cliffs, Prentice-Hall, 1993, pp 459-466.
151.
Ghoneim MM, Hinrichs JV, Mewaldt SP: Dose-response analysis of the behavioral effects of diazepam: I. Learning and Memory. Psychopharmacology 1984; 82:291-5.
152.
Ghoneim MM, Mewaldt SP, Hinrichs JV: Dose-response analysis of the behavioral effects of diazepam: II. Psychomotor performance, cognition and mood. Psychopharmacology 1984; 82:296-300.