Background

The authors evaluated a device designed to provide conscious sedation with propofol (propofol-air), or propofol combined with 50% nitrous oxide (N2O; propofol-N2O). An element of this device is the automated responsiveness test (ART), a method for confirming that patients remain conscious. The authors tested the hypotheses that the ART predicts loss of consciousness and that failure to respond to the ART precedes sedation-induced respiratory or hemodynamic toxicity.

Methods

The protocol consisted of sequential 15-min cycles in 20 volunteers. After a 15-min control period, propofol was infused to an initial target effect-site concentration of 0.0 microg/ml with N2O or 1.5 microg/ml with air. Subsequently, the propofol target effect-site concentration was increased by a designated increment (0.25 and 0.5 microg/ml) and the process repeated. This sequence was continued until loss of consciousness, as defined by an Observer's Assessment of Alertness/Sedation (OAA/S) score of 10/20 or less, or until an adverse physiologic event was detected.

Results

The OAA/S score at which only 50% of the volunteers were able to respond to the ART (P50) during propofol-N2O was 11.1 of 20 (95% confidence interval [CI]: 10.6-11.8); the analogous P50 was 11.8 of 20 (95% CI: 11.4-12.3) with propofol-air. Failure to respond to the ART occurred at a plasma propofol concentration of 0.7 +/- 0.6 microg/ml with propofol-N2O and 1.6 +/- 0.6 microg/ml with propofol-air, whereas loss of consciousness occurred at 1.2 +/- 0.8 microg/ml and 1.9 +/- 0.7 microg/ml, respectively. There were no false-normal ART responses.

Conclusion

The ART can guide individual titration of propofol because failure to respond to responsiveness testing precedes loss of consciousness and is not susceptible to false-normal responses. The use of N2O with propofol for conscious sedation decreases the predictive accuracy of the ART.

SEDATIVES and analgesics are commonly given to patients undergoing medical procedures. 1,2Conscious sedation 3can be effective, practical, and safe in an ambulatory setting. 4American Society of Anesthesiologists (ASA) guidelines specify that patients given conscious sedation by nonanesthesia personnel must remain arousable. 3The American Academy of Pediatric Dentistry similarly specifies that “loss of consciousness should be unlikely . . . and [that] the drugs and techniques used should carry a margin of safety wide enough to render unintended loss of consciousness unlikely.”5 

Nitrous oxide (N2O) is a short-acting drug that alone rarely produces important respiratory or hemodynamic complications. 6However, N2O alone is often insufficient. The gas is therefore frequently combined with opioids or sedatives. Because propofol combines the advantages of excellent sedation, some analgesia, 7and antiemetic action, 8it is a reasonable adjunct to N2O administration for conscious sedation.

We evaluated a device designed to provide conscious sedation with propofol alone or profopol combined with 50% N2O. The Vigilant Care System (Scott Laboratories, Inc., Lubbock, TX) includes three major elements: (1) all routine anesthetic monitors, including monitors for oscillometric blood pressure, electrocardiography, end-tidal partial pressure of carbon dioxide [Pco2], and oxygen saturation; (2) a computer-controlled propofol infusion system; and (3) an automated responsiveness test (ART). In the responsiveness testing system, a computer-generated voice instructs subjects to press a button. Ability to press the button within 10 s is considered presumptive evidence of consciousness.

The primary hypothesis was that failure to activate the automated responsiveness system predicts loss of consciousness and adverse physiologic events during sedation with propofol and propofol combined with N2O. Corollary hypotheses were that an unconscious subject would not reflexively respond during the 10-s request period (i.e. , a false-normal response) and that conscious but deeply sedated subjects would be able to activate the system (i.e. , lack of false-abnormal responses).

With approval of the University of California–San Francisco Committee on Human Research, we evaluated 20 healthy volunteers of either sex. Age was restricted to 18–40 yr. Volunteers fasted at least 4 h before the study. They were positioned comfortably on an operating room table and flexed into a chaise-lounge position.

Targeted Drug Delivery

The drug delivery system consisted of a Harvard 2 (Harvard Clinical Technology, South Natick, MA) electronic syringe pump and a customized software driver (Scott Laboratories, Inc.). We used a three-compartment model and published pharmacokinetic data. 9Our predicted plasma and effect-site concentrations were compared with a validated standard, StanPUMP 10(copyright S. Shafer 1986–1995; revision May 11, 1996; steven.shafer@stanford.edu; Palo Alto, CA). The predicted StanPUMP and the plasma and effect-site concentrations of our software driver were virtually identical. The system targeted effect-site concentrations rather than plasma concentrations. The target-controlled infusion system was monitored in real-time by graphical and numerical presentation of the predicted plasma and effect-site propofol concentrations on the screen of the Vigilant Care System monitor.

Protocol

The Vigilant Care System monitors were applied to the participating volunteers, as were other study monitors. The ART apparatus was strapped loosely to the dominant hand of the volunteer. A 20-gauge venous catheter was inserted into the contralateral arm above the wrist. An 18-gauge catheter for blood sampling was inserted at the antecubital fossa on the dominant arm. Lactated Ringer’s solution (200 ml) was infused as a bolus; subsequently, fluid was infused at a rate of 100 ml/h. Forced-air warming was used to maintain tympanic membrane temperature between 37.0 and 37.5°C.

The volunteers were introduced to the ART system for 10–15 min before each sedation trial. The audible volume of the ART was adjusted to a level that they were able to hear easily. We confirmed that the volunteers responded promptly to the ART during this prestudy period.

Volunteers were initially studied during administration of propofol (Diprivan 1%, Zeneca Inc., Wilmington, DE) and 50% N2O in oxygen (propofol–N2O). This combination was delivered via  the rebreathing circuit of a standard anesthesia machine (Modulus CD Anesthesia System; Ohmeda, Inc., Salt Lake City, UT) through a standard anesthesia mask. After a recovery period of at least 1 h, the volunteers were reevaluated using propofol and air–oxygen (propofol–air). Oxygen was administered at a concentration of 30%via  a sealed anesthesia mask during the propofol-only trial and throughout both recovery periods. The propofol–N2O always preceded the propofol–air trial because we wanted to avoid great accumulation of propofol in the plasma from the previous administration before starting N2O administration.

The basic protocol consisted of sequential 15-min cycles. After a 15-min-long control period without drugs, another 15-min cycle was tested with N2O alone, and then propofol was infused to an initial target effect-site concentration. The propofol infusion was maintained throughout each 15-min cycle until an outcome was reached and then, after a 1 h recovery period, the propofol–air trial started. Pharmacokinetic modeling indicated that steady state effect-site concentrations would be obtained within 4 min. Consequently, we considered the period from 9 to 15 elapsed min within each cycle to be at steady state. Subsequently, the propofol target effect-site concentration was increased, by a designated increment and the process was repeated.

Based on previous studies, 11–14we started at a target effect-site concentration of 1.5 μg/ml during the propofol–air trial, and then incremented the target concentration by 0.5 μg/ml. In contrast, the initial target propofol concentration during N2O administration was zero, and the concentration was increased by increments of 0.25 μg/ml.

The sequence of 15-min cycles at progressively greater propofol concentrations was continued until loss of consciousness was detected by an OAA/S score (see Measurements) of 10/20 or less (i.e. , no response to the verbal command), “red-alert” limits were reached (table 1), or a loss of response to the ART was observed. In either of the first two cases, propofol or the combination of propofol and N2O was discontinued, and the recovery portion of the study began. When the trigger was a loss of response to the ART, sedative drugs were discontinued only after the investigator confirmed that the volunteer was unconscious as determined by an OAA/S score of 10/20 or less.

Table 1. Yellow and Red Alerts and Intervention Limits

Autonomic responses defining yellow and red alerts, and intervention limits.

BP = blood pressure; PETco2= end-tidal carbon dioxide; Pso2= oxygen saturation determined by pulse oximetry.

Table 1. Yellow and Red Alerts and Intervention Limits
Table 1. Yellow and Red Alerts and Intervention Limits

The anesthesiologist took actions necessary to restore acceptable physiologic values only when the volunteers exceeded specific intervention limits (table 1). In keeping with good clinical practices, intervention was graded and based on need. Thus, inadequate ventilation or low oxygen saturation, for example, prompted repositioning of the head. If this proved to be insufficient, mild airway support was instituted, followed by moderate airway support or controlled ventilation.

Measurements

The ART consisted of a button incorporated into a handpiece that was linked to a computer-generated voice that instructed subjects via  an earpiece to press the button. This request was repeated up to 5 times over a 10-s interval. With each repetition, the voice became louder and more insistent. Furthermore, the instructions were accompanied by progressively more vigorous vibration of the handpiece. The voice and the vibration both stopped immediately when the button was pressed. Failure to activate the ART button within 10 s after the request was considered a nonresponse and was displayed by the Vigilant Care System as a “yellow alert.”

Demographic and morphometric characteristics of the volunteers were recorded. All routine physiologic values from the Vigilant Care System were digitally recorded. These included heart rate (HR), noninvasive arterial blood pressure (systolic arterial pressure, diastolic arterial pressure, mean arterial pressure [MAP]), end-tidal Pco2(Petco2) and N2O, respiratory rate (RR), and oxygen saturation (Pso2). Routine anesthetic data from each system were recorded as 1-min averages. The Petco2, oxygen, and N2O were sampled from a sealed anesthesia mask by two different sample lines, one of which was directed to the Vigilant Care System monitor and the other to a Rascal II (Ohmeda, Inc., Salt Lake City, UT). However, noninvasive arterial blood pressure was measured and recorded at 2-min intervals during sedation, as was ART; however, both were switched to 3-min cycles during recovery. Volunteers were monitored closely for potential episodes of excitement or uncooperative behavior.

As in previous studies, 15level of sedation was assessed using the OAA/S score (table 2). The OAA/S test consists of four components; as described by Chernik et al ., 16we summed the component scores. The OAA/S score sum is at least as reliable and valid as the visual analog scale and the digit–symbol substitution test. Sedation was always evaluated by the same investigator, who was not blind to the presence of N2O or to the target effect-site propofol concentration. The scoring system was applied after the ART result was obtained after the 9 and 15 min of each concentration cycle to minimize the effect of verbal or tactile stimulation on the ART. The scoring system was also applied whenever results of the ART indicated loss of consciousness or when the physiologic values reached a red-alert limit. Scores exceeding 10/20 were considered to be evidence of consciousness.

Table 2. Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation Scale

The final score is the sum of the responsiveness, speech, facial expression, and eyes component scores. Therefore, a “wide-awake” score = 20/20 and a “deeply sedated” score = 9/20 (1 [responsiveness]+ 2 [speech]+ 3 [faces]+ 3 [eyes]).

Table 2. Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation Scale
Table 2. Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation Scale

After 9 min of propofol administration at each target effect-site concentration, a venous blood sample for propofol concentration was obtained. Six minutes later, an additional sample was obtained to confirm steady state plasma concentrations. Samples were also obtained when loss of response to the ART and loss of consciousness were recognized and when the recovery score (see Measurements, last paragraph) first equaled or exceeded 12. All blood samples were obtained without tactile stimulation of the volunteers, and always after recording the results of the ART and OAA/S test.

Blood samples (5 ml) were stored in sodium fluoride tubes. These tubes were kept at 4°C for up to 10 weeks (propofol blood concentrations decrease less than 0.2%/week at 4°C). The samples were subsequently analyzed using a high-performance liquid chromatography assay modified from the method of Plummer. 17 

Nausea was evaluated by the subjects using a 4-point scale (none, mild, moderate, and severe). The volunteers were queried before drug administration and after 30 and 60 min of recovery. We also recorded the number of episodes of vomiting during drug administration and during the recovery period.

Fitness for discharge was evaluated at 3-min intervals using a modification of the Aldrete and Kroulik scoring system. 18The time elapsed between discontinuation of drug administration and a recovery score of 12 (85%) was considered to be the recovery duration.

Data Analysis

The seven sets of autonomic responses and three sets of blood pressures and ART results obtained during each 6-min steady state propofol infusion (9–15 min of each concentration cycle) were averaged for each volunteer. These values were then averaged among the participating volunteers.

Plasma propofol concentrations, hemodynamic and respiratory responses were averaged among the volunteers who reached loss of response to the ART and loss of consciousness with and without N2O. Recovery duration after each trial and the propofol concentration at recovery were similarly averaged among the volunteers who reached loss of consciousness. The sedation level, according to OAA/S score, and the hemodynamic and respiratory responses with and without N2O of the volunteers who reached a red alert were presented individually.

Logistic regressions were used to model the probability of loss of response to the ART as a function of the OAA/S score separately for the two sedation trials, with the generalized estimating equations approach 19used to account for possible dependence among multiple measurements from the same person. The effect of the OAA/S score was modeled as a linear term unless higher order terms (quadratic or cubic) produced better fit at P < 0.05. The probability of loss of response to the ART at an OAA/S score of 10/20 or less was estimated separately using exact binomial calculations with one point for each subject.

Plasma propofol concentrations at 9 and 15 min of each concentration increment were compared with paired t  tests to evaluate the validity of our steady state assumption. All data were presented as the mean and standard deviation. P < 0.05 were considered to be statistically significant.

The volunteers were aged 27 ± 6 yr, weighed 66 ± 10 kg, and were 169 ± 8 cm. Seven of 20 were men. The sedation period was 114 ± 46 min during the propofol–N2O sedation, but only 58 ± 22 min during the propofol–air sedation. Plasma propofol concentrations at the beginning and end of each steady state period (9 and 15 elapsed min) differed by only 0.0 ± 0.2 μg/ml (P = 0.85).

Progressive increases in the propofol plasma concentration during propofol–N2O and propofol–air trials induced changes in the cardiorespiratory physiology and are shown in figure 1. The OAA/S score at which only 50% of the volunteers were able to respond to the ART (P50) occurred at 11.1/20 (95% CI: 10.6–11.8) during propofol–N2O and of 11.8/20 (95% CI: 11.4–12.3) during propofol–air.

Fig. 1. Respiratory and hemodynamic responses and Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation (OAA/S) score at each steady state propofol concentration. Propofol–N2O and propofol–air distinguish between the trials with and without N2O. OAA/S score decreased linearly during propofol–N2O sedation: OAA/S score =−3.7 [propofol]+ 18.5, r2= 0.88; the score also decreased linearly during propofol–air: OAA/S score =−4.3 [propofol]+ 20.2, r2= 0.99. Results are presented as the mean ± SD. HR = heart rate; MAP = mean arterial pressure; Petco2= pressure of end-tidal carbon dioxide; Pso2= oxygen saturation as determined from a pulse oximeter.

Fig. 1. Respiratory and hemodynamic responses and Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation (OAA/S) score at each steady state propofol concentration. Propofol–N2O and propofol–air distinguish between the trials with and without N2O. OAA/S score decreased linearly during propofol–N2O sedation: OAA/S score =−3.7 [propofol]+ 18.5, r2= 0.88; the score also decreased linearly during propofol–air: OAA/S score =−4.3 [propofol]+ 20.2, r2= 0.99. Results are presented as the mean ± SD. HR = heart rate; MAP = mean arterial pressure; Petco2= pressure of end-tidal carbon dioxide; Pso2= oxygen saturation as determined from a pulse oximeter.

Close modal

Failure to respond to the ART occurred at a plasma propofol concentration of 0.7 ± 0.6 μg/ml (95% CI: 0.5–1.0) with propofol–N2O and of 1.6 ± 0.6 μg/ml (95% CI: 1.3–1.9) with propofol–air, whereas loss of consciousness, as defined by an OAA/S score of 10/20 or less, occurred at 1.2 ± 0.8 μg/ml (95% CI: 0.8–1.6) and 1.9 ± 0.7 μg/ml (95% CI: 1.5–2.3). Speech, facial expression, and eye signs seen on the OAA/S score disappeared well before the volunteers became unresponsive. Loss of consciousness was therefore invariably defined by the responsiveness component of the OAA/S test. Hemodynamic and respiratory responses remained essentially normal at loss of response to the ART and loss of consciousness (table 3).

Table 3. Plasma Propofol Concentrations and Respiratory and Hemodynamic Responses at Failure in Automated Responsiveness Test (ART) and Loss of Consciousness

Propofol–N2O and propofol–air distinguish between the trials with and without nitrous oxide.

HR = heart rate; SAP = systolic arterial pressure; PETco2= end-tidal carbon dioxide; Pso2= oxygen saturation determined by pulse oximetry.

Table 3. Plasma Propofol Concentrations and Respiratory and Hemodynamic Responses at Failure in Automated Responsiveness Test (ART) and Loss of Consciousness
Table 3. Plasma Propofol Concentrations and Respiratory and Hemodynamic Responses at Failure in Automated Responsiveness Test (ART) and Loss of Consciousness

Low respiratory rates were the only cause of red alerts; most were associated with partial airway obstruction, as defined by paradoxical chest movement and the subjects’ capnographic evaluation. However, only a single intervention was necessary (simple head repositioning), and oxygen saturation never decreased to less than 91% (table 4). Only six episodes of loss of consciousness and two red-alert events occurred within the first 3 min of a 15-min concentration cycle. More than 70% of the red alerts occurred between the P50for the loss of response to the ART and loss of consciousness. Only one red alert occurred before the P50for the loss of response to the ART in each trial (fig. 2). No responses to the ART occurred in patients who were truly unconscious, as determined by the OAA/S score.

Table 4. Individual Red Alerts and Their Corresponding Physiologies

OAA/S = Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation; HR = heart rate; SAP = systolic arterial pressure; RR = respiratory rate; PETco2= end-tidal carbon dioxide; Pso2= oxygen saturation measured by pulse oximetry.

Table 4. Individual Red Alerts and Their Corresponding Physiologies
Table 4. Individual Red Alerts and Their Corresponding Physiologies

Fig. 2. The probability of nonresponsiveness to the automated responsiveness test (ART) as a function of sedation level as defined by the Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation (OAA/S) score. Propofol–N2O and propofol–air sedation distinguish between the trials with and without N2O. The model fit best with a linear term for propofol–air and with a cubic term for propofol–N2O. The dashed lines represent the upper and lower 95% confidence bounds for each curve. The OAA/S score at a 50% probability for loss of responsiveness the ART (P50) is also indicated in each trial. The loss of consciousness is indicated by an OAA/S score equal to 10/20. Red (closed circles) and yellow (open circles) alerts were also indicated.

Fig. 2. The probability of nonresponsiveness to the automated responsiveness test (ART) as a function of sedation level as defined by the Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation (OAA/S) score. Propofol–N2O and propofol–air sedation distinguish between the trials with and without N2O. The model fit best with a linear term for propofol–air and with a cubic term for propofol–N2O. The dashed lines represent the upper and lower 95% confidence bounds for each curve. The OAA/S score at a 50% probability for loss of responsiveness the ART (P50) is also indicated in each trial. The loss of consciousness is indicated by an OAA/S score equal to 10/20. Red (closed circles) and yellow (open circles) alerts were also indicated.

Close modal

Forty-nine yellow alerts occurred during both sedation phases, and most (33) occurred in a single volunteer who had a slow baseline heart rate and a consistently low heart rate throughout the propofol–N2O trial; the remaining yellow alerts occurred because of low respiratory rate (9), low blood pressure (4), low oxygen saturation, from 90 to 93% (2), and low heart rate (1).

Most volunteers reached a recovery score of 12 (85%) within 3 min, and all were rated as fit for discharge within 6 min. Plasma propofol concentrations at a recovery score of 12 averaged 0.5 ± 0.5 μg/ml after propofol–N2O and 0.9 ± 0.5 μg/ml after propofol–air. Four red alerts and eight yellow alerts occurred during both recovery periods. All red alerts were respiratory in origin and associated with partial airway obstruction. Yellow alerts were associated with low blood pressure (4), low respiratory rate (3), and low heart rate (1).

One volunteer was mildly nauseated during the propofol–N2O trial and one was severely nauseated and vomited. Two volunteers became excited during the propofol–N2O trial. During sedation, most volunteers experienced altered sensation, perception, or mentation. These included paresthesia, numbness and tingling (which often occurred in the fingertips, toes, and perioral area), acoustic hypersensitivity, and a sense of floating or body acceleration “like being on a roller coaster.” None of the volunteers during the propofol–air trial became nauseated or excited. No nausea or vomiting occurred during the 1-h recovery period after either trial.

Loss of response to the ART consistently occurred before loss of consciousness. This difference was approximately 1 OAA/S unit and a 0.5-μg/ml plasma concentration of propofol during propofol–N2O sedation; it was 2 OAA/S units and a 0.3-μg/ml plasma concentration of propofol with propofol–air sedation. Importantly, the majority of sedation red-alert events occurred within this range, indicating that the loss of response to ART provides a reliable warning that unconsciousness will develop at only slightly greater propofol concentrations, and that greater concentrations are associated with a higher risk of adverse hemodynamic or respiratory complications.

The P50for loss of response to the ART occurred closer to loss of consciousness (as judged by the OAA/S score) during propofol–N2O sedation than during propofol–air sedation. However, loss of response to responsiveness testing began at an OAA/S score of 20/20 (fully awake) when N2O was included. This is consistent with previous reports that 50% N2O in oxygen has a dissociative action at various levels within the central nervous system. This causes muscle relaxation and consequent difficulty initiating motor tasks; psychomotor performance 20and some attention tasks 21are also adversely affected.

A nonresponse to verbal command followed by a response to mild physical stimulation has been used extensively to indicate loss of consciousness. 22–24This endpoint corresponds to an OAA/S sum of 10/20. Reliability of the OAA/S sum is at least as great as the OAA/S composite score during sedation. 16 

Previous reports indicate that loss of consciousness occurs at plasma propofol concentrations between 1.0 and 1.8 μg/ml. 11,12,14Casati et al.  25also report that unresponsiveness to mild prodding or shaking occurred at a target propofol concentration of 2 μg/ml. We similarly found that the loss of consciousness with propofol alone occurred at a plasma propofol concentration of 1.9 μg/ml. However, there was considerable variability in the associated propofol concentrations at loss of consciousness. These relatively large ranges suggest that plasma concentrations that are sufficiently low to avoid loss of consciousness in most of the population may provide inadequate sedation for many others.

The few red-alert and loss-of-consciousness events that occurred within the first 3 min of a concentration cycle may have been associated with an initial overshooting of the effect-site propofol concentration. Only 8 min of red alerts were observed during a total of 3,423 min of sedation (0.2%). All red alerts resulted from low respiratory rates. However, a better clinical criterion for “potentially serious” states would be those necessitating physician intervention. Only a single event qualified, requiring simple head repositioning, and even then oxygen saturation exceeded 90%.

Most of the red-alert events (five of eight) occurred during the propofol–N2O trial. Nitrous oxide is a mild respiratory stimulant. 26However, reduction of Petco2from hyperventilation in the presence of N2O can cause apnea, which,if untreated, can lead to hypoxemia. 27Despite its weak anesthetic effect, N2O has been shown to affect upper airway muscle function and produce obstructive phenomena. 28Beydon et al.  29also report that 50% N2O in oxygen induces longer and deeper sleep with more frequent respiratory events of central and peripheral type than does 50% O2. The combination of N2O with propofol may thus produce a sleep physiology prone to apneic–hypopneic events. Because N2O aggravated physiologic responses and reduced our ability to predict unconsciousness, we conclude that combining N2O with propofol for conscious sedation will increase response variability and hamper accurate titration of drug concentrations based on individual responses.

Recovery was rapid with or without N2O, averaging only 4 min. Nausea and vomiting were restricted to the propofol–N2O trial, and the incidence was 10%, which is consistent with previous reports. 30Excitement-increased sensory perception and perceptual illusion are well-known complications of N2O 30,31and were only observed during propofol–N2O sedation. Some respiratory red alerts occurred (0.2%), but no serious hemodynamic complications occurred after administration of the drug was discontinued. Furthermore, all the red-alert events occurred within the first 4 min of recovery. We therefore conclude that serious respiratory or hemodynamic events are rare and occur early during recovery after propofol–air or propofol–N2O sedation. However, it is likely that variability in the responses and sedation-induced toxicity will be greater in actual patients than in our uniform volunteer population.

We never observed false-normal ART results. Therefore, there were no potentially dangerous episodes during which the ART result incorrectly indicated that the volunteer was conscious. The ART thus errors on the side of safety, as defined by maintaining consciousness.

We conclude that the ART, with a negative predictive value for loss of consciousness of 100%, can serve as a basis for individual titration of conscious sedation with propofol. Serious respiratory or hemodynamic events are rare before or concurrent with loss of response to the ART, especially during propofol–air sedation. The major outcome is this: it is safer to stop augmenting depth of sedation when response to ART is lost rather than continue until actual loss of consciousness. The use of N2O with propofol for conscious sedation increases the risk of adverse physiologic responses and other adverse events, such as nausea and excitement, and decreases the predictive accuracy of the ART.

The authors thank the following for assistance: Randy Hickle, M.D., Brett Moore, B.S., and Jason Derouen, B.S. (all from Scott Laboratories, Inc., Lubbock, TX), and Angela Rajek, M.D., Department of Anesthesia, University of California—San Francisco.

1.
Balsells F, Wyllie R, Kay M, Steffen R: Use of conscious sedation for lower and upper gastrointestinal endoscopic examinations in children, adolescents, and young adults: A twelve-year review. Gastrointest Endosc 1997; 45: 375–80
2.
Woolard DJ, Terndrup TE: Sedative-analgesic agent administration in children: Analysis of use and complications in the emergency department. J Emerg Med 1994; 12: 453–61
3.
Gross JB, Bailey PL, Caplan RA, Connis RT, Cote CJ, Davis FG, Epstein BS, Kapur PA, Zerwas JM, Zuccaro G Jr: Practice guidelines for sedation and analgesia by non-anesthesiologists. A report by the American Society of Anesthesiologists Task Force on Sedation and Analgesia by Non-Anesthesiologists. A nesthesiology 1996; 84: 459–71
4.
Odom J: Conscious sedation in the ambulatory setting. Crit Care Nurs Clin North Am 1997; 9: 361–70
5.
American Academy of Pediatric Dentistry: Guidelines for the elective use of conscious sedation, deep sedation, and general anesthesia in pediatric patients. Committee on drugs. Section on anesthesiology. Pediatrics 1985; 76: 317–21
American Academy of Pediatric Dentistry:
6.
Griffin GC, Campbell VD, Jones R: Nitrous oxide–oxygen sedation for minor surgery. Experience in a pediatric setting. JAMA 1981; 245: 2411–3
7.
Anker-Moller E, Spangsberg N, Arendt-Nielsen L, Schultz P, Kristensen MS, Bjerring P: Subhypnotic doses of thiopentone and propofol cause analgesia to experimentally induced acute pain. Br J Anaesth 1991; 66: 185–8
8.
Gan TJ, Ginsberg B, Grant AP, Glass PS: Double-blind, randomized comparison of ondansetron and intraoperative propofol to prevent postoperative nausea and vomiting. A nesthesiology 1996; 85: 1036–42
9.
Schnider TW, Minto CF, Gambus PL, Andresen C, Goodale DB, Shafer SL, Youngs EJ: The influence of method of administration and covariates on the pharmacokinetics of propofol in adult volunteers. A nesthesiology 1998; 88: 1170–82
10.
Raemer DB, Buschman A, Varvel JR, Philip BK, Johnson MD, Stein DA, Shafer SL: The prospective use of population pharmacokinetics in a computer-driven infusion system for alfentanil. A nesthesiology 1990; 73: 66–72
11.
Irwin MG, Thompson N, Kenny GN: Patient-maintained propofol sedation. Assessment of a target-controlled infusion system. Anaesthesia 1997; 52: 525–30
12.
Fiset P, Paus T, Daloze T, Plourde G, Meuret P, Bonhomme V, Hajj-Ali N, Backman SB, Evans AC: Brain mechanisms of propofol-induced loss of consciousness in humans: A positron emission tomographic study. J Neurosci 1999; 19: 5506–13
13.
Skipsey IG, Colvin JR, Mackenzie N, Kenny GN: Sedation with propofol during surgery under local blockade. Assessment of a target-controlled infusion system. Anaesthesia 1993; 48: 210–3
14.
Oei-Lim VL, Kalkman CJ, Makkes PC, Ooms WG: Patient-controlled versus anesthesiologist-controlled conscious sedation with propofol for dental treatment in anxious patients. Anesth Analg 1998; 86: 967–72
15.
Rampil IJ, Kim JS, Lenhardt R, Negishi C, Sessler DI: Bispectral EEG index during nitrous oxide administration. A nesthesiology 1998; 89: 671–7
16.
Chernik DA, Gillings D, Laine H, Hendler J, Silver JM, Davidson AB, Schwam EM, Siegel JL: Validity and reliability of the Observer’s Assessment of Alertness/Sedation Scale: Study with intravenous midazolam. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1990; 10: 244–51
17.
Plummer GF: Improved method for the determination of propofol in blood by high- performance liquid chromatography with fluorescence detection. J Chromatogr 1987; 421: 171–6
18.
Aldrete JA, Kroulik D: A postanesthetic recovery score. Anesth Analg 1970; 49: 924–34
19.
Liang KY, Zeger SL: Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika 1986; 73: 13–22
20.
Janiszewski DJ, Galinkin JL, Klock PA, Coalson DW, Pardo H, Zacny JP: The effects of subanesthetic concentrations of sevoflurane and nitrous oxide, alone and in combination, on analgesia, mood, and psychomotor performance in healthy volunteers. Anesth Analg 1999; 88: 1149–54
21.
Pang EW, Fowler B: Dissociation of the mismatch negativity and processing negativity attentional waveforms with nitrous oxide. Psychophysiology 1999; 36: 552–8
22.
Flaishon R, Windsor A, Sigl J, Sebel PS: Recovery of consciousness after thiopental or propofol. Bispectral index and isolated forearm technique. A nesthesiology 1997; 86: 613–9
23.
Kearse LA Jr, Rosow C, Zaslavsky A, Connors P, Dershwitz M, Denman W: Bispectral analysis of the electroencephalogram predicts conscious processing of information during propofol sedation and hypnosis. A nesthesiology 1998; 88: 25–34
24.
Dunnet JM, Prys-Roberts C, Holland DE, Browne BL: Propofol infusion and the suppression of consciousness: dose requirements to induce loss of consciousness and to suppress response to noxious and non-noxious stimuli. Br J Anaesth 1994; 72: 29–34
25.
Casati A, Fanelli G, Casaletti E, Colnaghi E, Cedrati V, Torri G: Clinical assessment of target-controlled infusion of propofol during monitored anesthesia care. Can J Anaesth 1999; 46: 235–9
26.
Royston D, Jordan C, Jones JG: Effect of subanaesthetic concentrations of nitrous oxide on the regulation of ventilation in man. Br J Anaesth 1983; 55: 449–55
27.
Northwood D, Sapsford DJ, Jones JG, Griffiths D, Wilkins C: Nitrous oxide sedation causes post-hyperventilation apnoea. Br J Anaesth 1991; 67: 7–12
28.
Nishino T, Takizawa K, Yokokawa N, Hiraga K: Depression of the swallowing reflex during sedation and/or relative analgesia produced by inhalation of 50% nitrous oxide in oxygen. A nesthesiology 1987; 67: 995–8
29.
Beydon L, Goldenberg F, Heyer L, d’Ortho MP, Bonnet F, Harf A, Lofaso F: Sleep apnea-like syndrome induced by nitrous oxide inhalation in normal men. Respir Physiol 1997; 108: 215–24
30.
Smith RA, Beirne OR: The use of nitrous oxide by dentists, Nitrous Oxide/N2O. Edited by Eger EI. New York, Elsevier, 1985, pp 281–304
31.
Fenwick P, Bushman J, Howard R, Perry I, Gamble T: Contingent negative variation and evoked potential amplitude as a function of inspired nitrous oxide concentration. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1979; 47: 473–82