Background

Despite significant advances in cardiopulmonary bypass (CPB) technology, surgical techniques, and anesthetic management, central nervous system complications occur in a large percentage of patients undergoing surgery requiring CPB. Many centers are switching to normothermic CPB because of shorter CPB and operating room times and improved myocardial protection. The authors hypothesized that, compared with normothermia, hypothermic CPB would result in superior neurologic and neurocognitive function after coronary artery bypass graft surgery.

Methods

Three hundred patients undergoing elective coronary artery bypass graft surgery were prospectively enrolled and randomly assigned to either normothermic (35.5-36.5 degrees C) or hypothermic (28-30 degrees C) CPB. A battery of neurocognitive tests was performed preoperatively and at 6 weeks after surgery. Four distinct cognitive domains were identified and standardized using factor analysis and were then compared on a continuous scale.

Results

Two hundred twenty-seven patients participated in 6-week follow-up testing. There were no differences in neurologic or neurocognitive outcomes between normothermic and hypothermic groups in multivariable models, adjusting for covariable effects of baseline cognitive function, age, and years of education, as well as interaction of these with temperature treatment.

Conclusions

Hypothermic CPB does not provide additional central nervous system protection in adult cardiac surgical patients who were maintained at either 30 or 35 degrees C during CPB.

CARDIAC surgery with cardiopulmonary bypass (CPB) is associated with a predictable incidence of central nervous system dysfunction that accounts for significant morbidity and mortality. 1–3Embolic events, changes in cerebral blood flow, global hypoperfusion, cerebral reperfusion injury, and a CPB-triggered whole body inflammatory response represent possible mechanisms. Elderly patients are more susceptible to cognitive impairment and stroke after cardiac surgery than others. 4Thus, with the increase in the percentage of elderly patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft (CABG) surgery, there has been a parallel increase in the incidence of stroke and neurocognitive dysfunction. 5,6Importantly, overall mortality after cardiac surgery has decreased over the past 20 yr, but adverse events attributed to neurologic deficits have increased. 7This may be a result of the fact that the vast improvements in myocardial protection during cardiac surgery occurred, but similar advances in neurologic protection have not.

In the last decade, several investigators have revived the use of systemic normothermia with warm cardioplegic solution during CPB. 8–10These investigators noted a higher rate of spontaneous defibrillation after cross-clamp removal, as well as a trend toward a lower rate of myocardial infarction and reduced use of the intraaortic balloon pump in the “warm heart” patients. Thus, systemic normothermia with warm cardioplegia represents a safe technique, suitable for high-risk patients undergoing cardiac surgery. 10However, the abandonment of the potentially neuroprotective action of hypothermia could affect cerebral protection during CPB. In this prospective, randomized study, we tested the hypothesis that hypothermia during CPB reduces the incidence and severity of postoperative neurocognitive dysfunction.

After institutional review board approval (Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC) and written informed consent were obtained, 300 patients undergoing elective CABG were enrolled in the study. Patients with history of cerebrovascular disease with residual deficits, uncontrolled hypertension, alcoholism, psychiatric illness, renal disease (creatinine concentration > 2 mg/dl), and active liver disease were excluded. Pregnant women and patients with less than a seventh-grade education were also excluded. Patients were randomly assigned before surgery to normothermic (35.5–36.5°C) CPB systemic perfusion (“warm group”) or hypothermic (28–30°C) CPB systemic perfusion (“cold group”). Both groups received intermittent hypothermic (8°C) antegrade blood cardioplegic solution for myocardial protection during CPB. Investigators performing the preoperative and postoperative assessments were blinded to the temperature assignment of each patient. Only the physicians directly involved with the intraoperative care of these patients were aware of group assignment.

Neurologic Testing

Complete neurologic examinations were performed by a Neurology Fellow preoperatively and on days 3–5 postoperatively. Neurologic function was rated based on the following: (1) clinical diagnosis of stroke or encephalopathy; (2) a single global rating with seven possible responses; and (3) by means of the Western Perioperative Neurologic Scale (WPNS). Stroke was defined as the fulfillment of any of the following criteria: (1) new motor or sensory deficits of the face or upper and lower extremities (not attributable to peripheral lesions); (2) impaired speech; (3) visual disturbances; or (4) encephalopathy defined as altered level of consciousness or confusion (not attributable to pharmacologic or metabolic causes) as the result of diffuse cerebral dysfunction, diagnosed from clinical judgment using data from neurologic examination. The global neurologic score provided the examiner’s impression of the patient’s general neurologic state: normal, minimal, mild, moderate, serious, severe, or vegetative. The WPNS is designed to detect and quantify anatomically discrete neurologic dysfunction. It includes 14 specific items in eight domains: mentation, speech, cranial nerve (two items), motor (four items), cerebellum–sensation (three items), reflexes (two items), and gait. Each item is scored from 0 (severe deficit) to 3 (normal), for a total possible score of 42.

For assessment of neurologic change, the amount of change in WPNS scores from before surgery to after surgery was used as a numeric measure of neurologic outcome. Discrete binary (i.e. , yes–no) neurologic outcomes included the following: (1) decline of two or more points on the WPNS, representing either mild decrease in performance on two items or significant decrease on one item; and (2) clinical evidence of new stroke, encephalopathy, and postoperative neurologic deficit on neurologic evaluation.

An overall “adverse neurologic outcome” variable was defined as the occurrence of any of the following discrete outcomes: (1) decline of two or more levels in the seven-level global assessment; (2) new stroke at discharge; (3) new encephalopathy at discharge; or (4) postoperative neurologic changes suggestive of a stroke (table 1). Stroke was a clinical outcome and not defined by computed tomography scans or magnetic resonance imaging.

Table 1. Adverse Neurologic Outcome

Table 1. Adverse Neurologic Outcome
Table 1. Adverse Neurologic Outcome

Neuropsychologic Testing

A neurocognitive battery was administered the day before surgery and at 6 weeks postoperatively. Cognitive functioning was assessed by several methods 11:

  • 1. The Short Story module of the Randt Memory Test requires subjects to recall the details of a short story immediately after it has been read to them (immediate) and after a 30-min delay;

  • 2. The Digit Span subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised Test requires subjects to repeat a series of digits that have been orally presented to them both forward and, in an independent test, in reverse order;

  • 3. The Digit Symbol subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised Test is a paper-and-pencil task that requires subjects to reproduce, within 90 s, as many coded symbols as possible in blank boxes beneath randomly generated digits according to a coding scheme for pairing digits with symbols;

  • 4. The Modified Visual Reproduction Test from the Wechsler Memory Scale measures short- and long-term figural memory and requires subjects to reproduce from memory several geometric shapes both immediately and after a 30-min delay 12;

  • 5. The Trail Making Test (Trails) (part B) requires subjects to connect, by drawing a line, a series of numbers and letters in sequence (i.e. , 1-A-2-B) as quickly as possible.

Anesthetic and Surgical Techniques

Patients were premedicated with 0.1 mg/kg diazepam and 0.1 mg/kg methadone orally 90 min before induction. Induction and maintenance of anesthesia was achieved with a continuous infusion of midazolam and fentanyl. 13Supplemental isoflurane (0.5–1.0%) was used as required to maintain heart rate and mean blood pressure within 25% of the preinduction values. Pancuronium was administered to achieve and maintain neuromuscular paralysis. The perfusion apparatus consisted of a Cobe CML oxygenator (COBE Chem Labs, Lakewood, CO), a Sarns 7000 max pump (Sarns Inc., 3M Inc., Ann Arbor, MI), and Pall SP 3840 arterial line filter (Pall Biomedical Products Co., Glen Cove, NY). Nonpulsatile perfusion was maintained at 2–2.4 l · min1· m2. The pump was primed with crystalloid solution (0.9% normal saline) designed to achieve a hematocrit of 0.18 or higher during extracorporeal circulation. Packed erythrocytes were added when necessary to maintain the desired hematocrit. All patients were perfused during CPB through an ascending aortic cannula. Arterial carbon dioxide tension was maintained throughout CPB at 35–40 mmHg (uncorrected for temperature), with the arterial oxygen tension maintained at 150–250 mmHg. Mean arterial pressure was maintained between 50 and 90 mmHg during CPB using intravenous phenylephrine or nitroprusside as required.

Patients enrolled in the normothermic group received a perfusate inflow temperature of 36°C and were actively rewarmed to a nasopharyngeal temperature of 37°C before separation from CPB using an inflow temperature of 38°C. The hypothermic group received an inflow temperature of 28°C and was also rewarmed to a nasopharyngeal temperature of 37°C after cross-clamp removal and before discontinuation of CPB per institutional guidelines. Maximal perfusate temperature was maintained at 38°C. Myocardial protection was achieved in both groups with cardioplegia delivered at 8°C. Myocardial temperature was maintained at 20°C or less during the period of aortic cross-clamping. Nasopharyngeal temperature and mean arterial pressure were measured each minute during CPB and recorded automatically using the Arkive®Information Management System (Arkive IMS Inc., San Diego, CA). Nasopharyngeal temperature measurements during the rewarming period were summarized as follows: (1) peak temperature; (2) duration (minutes) of temperature greater than 37°C; and (3) area under the curve for the period during which the mean temperature was greater than 37°C.

Statistical Analysis

Baseline and demographic characteristics were compared using the Fisher exact test for categoric variables and the t  test or Wilcoxon rank sum test for numeric variables. Significance was assessed at a two-tailed α of 0.05.

To assess neurocognitive decline over time while minimizing the potential for neurocognitive testing overlap, a factor analysis with orthogonal rotation was performed on the eight baseline neurocognitive measures obtained from the five neurocognitive instruments for the entire population. 14,15This method finds the commonality (overlap in testing) among the set of raw scores and constructs a smaller set of independent factor scores, each representing a separate domain of cognitive function. The four factors represent the following cognitive domains: (1) verbal memory and language comprehension, short-term and delayed; (2) figural or visual memory, short-termed and delayed; (3) attention and concentration; and (4) visuospatial orientation, psychomotor processing speed, and attention. Factor scores for both baseline and 6-week cognitive function were calculated using the factor loadings and weights from the baseline analysis. In this manner, the factors (cognitive domains) were identified at preoperative baseline testing and remained consistent for the 6-week test period.

An overall (summary) cognitive function score at each test period was determined by adding together the independent factor scores. A cognitive change score (cognitive index) was calculated by subtracting the baseline from the 6-week score for each of the factors and for the overall score, thus representing a continuous measure of cognitive assessment. In addition, a binary cognitive deficit outcome event was defined as a decline in performance of 1 SD or more in any of the independent factors (domains). The baseline summary score was used to control for baseline cognitive function in multivariable models.

The effect of temperature treatment on 6-week change in cognitive function scores was investigated with linear regression models. The effect of temperature treatment on the occurrence of cognitive deficit at 6 weeks was investigated with logistic regression models. Although the analysis of neurocognitive performance as a continuous measure is more sensitive to improvement, the analysis of neurocognitive deficit as a dichotomous measure captures only serious decline. As a first step in all analyses, the unadjusted association between treatment and outcome (cognitive function scores and cognitive deficit as dependent variables) was tested either by a simple linear correlation or Fisher exact test. Then multivariable models were used for both linear and logistic regression models to test and adjust for covariable effects of baseline cognitive function, age, years of education, diabetes, left ventricular ejection fraction, as well as the interactions of these with temperature treatment. Nonsignificant covariables were dropped stepwise from the models, starting with interactions.

For neurologic assessment, the amount of change at predischarge in WPNS scores was calculated by subtracting the baseline score from the predischarge score. These change scores were used as numeric measures of neurologic outcome. Discrete binary (i.e. , yes–no) neurologic outcomes included:

  • 1. decline of two or more points on the WPNS;

  • 2. clinical evidence of new stroke, encephalopathy, and postoperative neurologic deficit on neurologic evaluation:

  • decline of two or more levels in the seven-level global assessment;

  • new stroke at discharge;

  • new encephalopathy at discharge; or

  • postoperative neurologic changes suggestive of a stroke.

An overall adverse neurologic outcome variable was defined as the occurrence of any of the discrete outcomes above. The effect of temperature treatment on predischarge neurologic outcome was investigated in a way similar to the cognitive outcome, with linear regression models on the WPNS change scores and logistic regression models on the binary neurologic outcomes.

Missing Data

Many statistical analysis methods, including factor analysis, require that patients with incomplete data be dropped from analysis. Rather than losing all psychometric data on patients who were missing only a few scores from a test battery, missing data were imputed using “place holder” values that do not affect group mean change scores but allow the rest of the patient’s data to be used in analysis. Patients missing both baseline and 6-week scores on a test were given the group mean for baseline, to which the mean change for that test was added for the 6-week score. Patients missing only the 6-week score were assigned their own baseline plus the mean change for that test for a 6-week score. Similarly, if only the baseline score was missing, it was imputed from the patient’s 6-week score minus the mean change for that test. No imputation was performed for patients who did not return for testing at 6 weeks or who were missing more than two of the eight scores from a testing period. Patients whose 6-week scores were missing because of death or stroke were given worst possible scores on 6-week tests. Data were analyzed both with and without these scores included. Patients who were not treated as assigned (because of the surgeon’s decision during the operation to change the CPB temperature) were analyzed as randomized (intention to treat) and in a separate follow-up analysis were analyzed based on treatment received.

A total of 300 patients undergoing elective CABG surgery were enrolled in the study: 298 completed preoperative testing, and 227 completed 6-week postoperative testing. Table 2shows that the warm and cold groups were similar with respect to preoperative and operative characteristics. Despite the randomization, the preoperative neurocognitive tests revealed slightly poorer performance in the cold group than the warm group (table 3).

Table 2. Demographic Characteristics of Warm and Cold Groups

* Using the Fisher exact test.

P  value for Wilcoxon Rank-Sum Test comparing groups.

ASA = American Society of Anesthesiologists; HTN = hypertension; LVEF = left ventricular ejection fraction; CPB = cardiopulmonary bypass.

Table 2. Demographic Characteristics of Warm and Cold Groups
Table 2. Demographic Characteristics of Warm and Cold Groups

Table 3. Raw Scores for Neurocognitive Test Battery by Treatment Group at Baseline and 6 Weeks Postoperatively

* Wilcoxon rank-sum P = 0.0501 for comparison of groups at baseline.

Table 3. Raw Scores for Neurocognitive Test Battery by Treatment Group at Baseline and 6 Weeks Postoperatively
Table 3. Raw Scores for Neurocognitive Test Battery by Treatment Group at Baseline and 6 Weeks Postoperatively

One hundred forty-nine patients were randomized to the warm CPB group, while 151 patients received hypothermic CPB. As intended, patients in the cold group had a lower mean nasopharyngeal temperature during CPB (30.4 ± 1.4°C) than patients in the warm group (35.1 ± 1.3°C) (P < 0.01;table 4, fig. 1). During CPB, because of surgical considerations, four patients (3%) in the cold group were kept at a temperature greater than 35°C, and 13 patients (9%) in the warm group were cooled to less than 32°C during CPB. These protocol violations occurred because of surgical requirements, i.e. , the operating surgeon requested the temperature to facilitate the operation. During the late phase of rewarming during CPB, the patients in the cold group had a higher mean peak temperature (37.6 ± 0.5°C vs.  37.0 ± 0.7°C;P < 0 .001), an increased mean duration of temperature greater than 37°C (28.2 ± 23.4 min vs.  15.5 ± 23.8 min;P < 0.001), and a higher mean temperature area greater than 37°C (11.4 ± 12.2°C-min vs.  5.5 ± 11.7°C-min;P < 0.001) than patients in the warm group.

Table 4. Physiologic Variables During Cardiopulmonary Bypass

Values reported are after cross-clamping the aorta.

*P  value for t  test comparing groups.

TempNP = nasopharyngeal temperature; MAP = mean arterial pressure; Pao2= radial arterial Po2; Paco2= radial arterial Pco2; Sao2= radial artery saturation; CVP = superior vena caval pressure; Pvco2= venous reservoir Pco2; Pvo2= venous reservoir Po2; Svo2= venous reservoir saturation.

Table 4. Physiologic Variables During Cardiopulmonary Bypass
Table 4. Physiologic Variables During Cardiopulmonary Bypass

Fig. 1. Mean temperature and SD in the early, middle, and late phases of cardiopulmonary bypass (CPB) by study group.

Fig. 1. Mean temperature and SD in the early, middle, and late phases of cardiopulmonary bypass (CPB) by study group.

Close modal

Baseline neurocognitive tests were completed in 147 patients in the warm group and 151 patients in the cold group. There were 32 patients in the warm group and 39 patients in cold group who missed the 6-week neurocognitive assessment after CABG and were therefore excluded (table 5). Two patients in the cold group were excluded from analysis for missing more than two of the eight scores from a testing period. Thus, at 6 weeks after CABG, 117 patients in the warm group and 110 patients in the cold group completed neurocognitive testing. A total of only 35 scores of 5,250 used in analysis (0.67%) were imputed. Factor analysis yielded four factors representing separate domains of cognitive function and accounting for 83% of the variance in the test battery: factor 1, verbal memory and language comprehension, short-term and delayed; factor 2, figural or visual memory, short-term and delayed; factor 3, attention and concentration; and factor 4, visuospatial orientation, psychomotor processing speed, and attention. The overall incidence of cognitive deficit (≥ 1 SD decline in any of four independent factors) was 39.3% (46 of 117) in the warm group and 39.1% (43 of 110) in the cold group (P = 0.99). There were no statistically significant differences between warm and cold groups in incidence or severity of cognitive decline at 6 weeks (table 6). The mean change in total cognitive index at 6 weeks was 0.341 ± 0.871 in the warm group and 0.348 ± 0.975 in the cold group (P = 0.89, univariate Wilcoxon rank sum test). Multivariate analyses of the incidence and severity of cognitive change, controlling for baseline cognitive function, age, years of education, diabetes, and left ventricular ejection fraction, revealed similar results.

Table 5. Reason for the Missed 6-Week Neurocognitive Assessment

Transportation = too difficult to get to the hospital for testing; interest = patient not interested in repeating testing; stress = patient found testing too stressful; health = patient did not feel well enough to participate; family = family responsibilities prevented testing; time = patient too busy; no contact = unable to contact patient for follow-up; no show = patient failed to keep testing appointment; other = patient expressed other reason for not participating.

Table 5. Reason for the Missed 6-Week Neurocognitive Assessment
Table 5. Reason for the Missed 6-Week Neurocognitive Assessment

Table 6. Mean, Standard Deviation, and Median of Continuous Cognitive Function Factor Scores by Treatment Group

Factor 1 = verbal memory and language comprehension, short-term and delayed; factor 2 = figural or visual memory, short-term and delayed; factor 3 = attention and concentration; factor 4 = visuo spatial orientation, psychomotor processing speed and attention.

Total factor score is the sum of the four independent factor scores; total 6-week factor change is the difference in mean factor score at 6 weeks compared with baseline; smaller total 6-week factor change indicates less improvement from baseline.

Table 6. Mean, Standard Deviation, and Median of Continuous Cognitive Function Factor Scores by Treatment Group
Table 6. Mean, Standard Deviation, and Median of Continuous Cognitive Function Factor Scores by Treatment Group

Neurologic outcome based on preoperative and postoperative neurologic examination was determined in 136 patients in the warm group and 134 patients in the cold group. The incidence of any adverse neurologic event was 14.7% (20 of 136) in the warm group compared with 13.4% (18 of 134) in the cold group (P = 0.86). Two strokes occurred in each group (1.32% in the cold group and 1.34% in the warm group;P = 0.7).

None of the patients enrolled in the warm group died or required insertion of intraaortic balloon pump. Of the total number of subjects enrolled in the cold group, three patients (1.9%) died, and two (1.3%) required insertion of intraaortic balloon pump. The difference between groups was not significant (P = 0.25 and 0.50 respectively). One patient in each group experienced postoperative cardiac arrest (P = 0.99). None of the patients enrolled in the study returned to the operating room for graft revision or uncontrolled bleeding. Results from all secondary analyses, including the analysis of groups as treated (instead of as randomized), did not differ from the primary analysis.

Our hypothesis that hypothermic CPB has neuroprotective effects reflected in a reduction of decreased cognitive function was not proven. Thus, the major finding of our study is that hypothermic CPB as practiced (at our institution with a temperature difference of 4.7°C) makes no difference in cognitive performance.

Hypothermia is used in heart, brain, and other organ protection during CPB. During hypothermic conditions, the rate of myocardial use of adenosine triphosphate and creatine phosphate is decreased secondary to a reduction in electromechanical and basal metabolic activity. 16Moderate hypothermia has been routinely used during CPB with the hope of providing organ protection, although mechanisms for this purported protection remain unclear. During hypothermic conditions, the rate of myocardial use of adenosine triphosphate is decreased secondary to a reduction in electromechanical and basal metabolic activity.

In the ischemic brain, moderate hypothermia reduces cerebral metabolic rate, 17blocks release of glutamate, 18reduces calcium influx, 19hastens recovery of protein synthesis, 20diminishes membrane-bound protein kinase C activity, 21slows time to onset of depolarization, 22reduces formation of reactive oxygen species, 23suppresses nitric oxide synthase activity, 24and inhibits spontaneous depolarizations. 25Consistent with this, either intraischemic or sustained postischemic moderate (or mild) hypothermia provide lasting reduction of brain injury in innumerable laboratory models of acute brain injury. Despite these findings, there remains no human evidence derived from prospective, randomized, appropriately powered studies that either mild or moderate hypothermia serves as a neuroprotectant against acute brain injury, although such studies are currently ongoing. 26Several smaller studies have identified interesting trends toward neuroprotection in humans suffering from closed head injury or undergoing cerebral aneurysm surgery, but sustained improvement in outcome measures did not reach statistical significance. 26,27 

There are, however, potential limitations to hypothermia efficacy during CPB. Although hypothermia decreases overall cellular metabolism, oxygen delivery is likewise decreased as the result of a leftward shift in the oxygen–hemoglobin dissociation curve. 28In addition, as previously discussed, a reduced temperature reduces cerebral blood flow. Hemodilution also reduces oxygen-carrying capacity of the blood, and the total oxygen delivery to the tissue may be diminished. Moreover, during obligate systemic rewarming, because nasopharyngeal temperatures underestimate jugular venous temperatures, the central nervous system could be exposed to supranormal temperatures, potentially accelerating ischemic cerebral injury. 29–32Other deleterious effects of hypothermic CPB include postbypass redistribution hypothermia, 33an increased risk of blood loss and transfusion requirements, 34reversible platelet dysfunction, 35impaired leukocyte and T-cell function, 36and increased metabolic rate and myocardial oxygen consumption as a result of shivering. 37 

The concept of warm heart surgery was partly proposed to reduce or prevent these harmful effects of hypothermia. 38Several studies have subsequently attempted to assess the effect of normothermic versus  hypothermic CPB on cerebral outcome in cardiac surgical patients (table 7). Although these investigators have looked at similar patient populations, results have been conflicting. Martin et al. , 39in a series of 1,001 patients, and Mora et al. , 40using data from a subgroup (n = 138) of this patient population, reported a significantly increased rate of neurologic dysfunction in patients undergoing warm (> 35°C) CPB compared with cold (< 28°C) CPB. In the study by Mora et al. , 40the patients enrolled in the normothermic group were older than those in the hypothermic group, thus predisposing them to a greater potential for neurologic dysfunction. 40Similarly, the warm group in the study by Martin et al.  39experienced longer periods of aortic cross-clamping. Moreover, temperature management during CPB was not well controlled, and active rewarming of the normothermic patients during all of CPB to bladder temperatures between 35 and 37°C might have achieved brain temperatures greater than 37°C.

Table 7. Studies Investigating the Effect of Temperature on Neurologic and Neurocognitive Dysfunction in Patients Undergoing CABG

CABG = coronary artery bypass grafting.

Table 7. Studies Investigating the Effect of Temperature on Neurologic and Neurocognitive Dysfunction in Patients Undergoing CABG
Table 7. Studies Investigating the Effect of Temperature on Neurologic and Neurocognitive Dysfunction in Patients Undergoing CABG

Alternatively, many investigators have reported no relation between perfusion temperature and neurologic outcome. 41–45These studies are also limited by the use of deliberate hyperthermia (39°C perfusion temperature) during rewarming, 41the use of hypothermic temperatures (34–35°C) in the warm group, 42a significantly higher duration of CPB and cross-clamp time in the cold group, 43the use of retrospective chart review for assessment of neurologic outcome, 44or a poorly controlled CPB temperature protocol. 45Furthermore, none of these studies have assessed the full array of neurologic and neurocognitive dysfunction after CPB used in this study.

In the current study, we used a strict temperature, blood gas, and perfusion pressure protocol during CPB (fig. 1). We elected to choose a wide separation in temperature groups (5°C) so that there was a relatively wide difference in temperature management of the two groups during CPB. Our study is the first to report peak and mean CPB temperatures during rewarming in postoperative cognitive outcomes. In addition, we assessed a broad spectrum of short-term postoperative central nervous system dysfunction using precise, previously validated neurologic and neurocognitive test batteries. Neurocognitive testing has been used to assess the onset and severity of dementia, 46Alzheimer disease, 47,48and recovery from stroke, 49,50as well as the efficacy of treatment strategies for Alzheimer disease and early cognitive decline. 51,52Several important consensus statements have identified key domains for assessment, 53,54but intervention strategies have varied in their ability to provide substantial protection from decline, similar to the variability of efficacy in stroke trials. 55–57However, recent studies have demonstrated important associations between neurocognitive function and quality of life after cardiac surgery, emphasizing the relevance of this end point. 58 

In an attempt to adequately assess neurocognitive decline over time while minimizing the potential for neurocognitive testing overlap, we chose to analyze the cognitive function with factor analysis. Factors are, by definition, uncorrelated with each other, and using factor domains as outcomes instead of separate test scores in analyses reduces the concern over redundancy of tests and the possibility of over-representing a single domain of cognitive functioning. Type I errors caused by multiple comparisons are also minimal, and analysis can be conducted on each of the factors as unique outcomes. Incorporating these methodologic improvements, we were unable to detect a beneficial effect of hypothermic CPB on neurocognitive function.

We chose factor analysis because we were faced with the difficulty of assessing eight overlapping cognitive performance scores (table 3) on each individual at each test period. Our goal was to investigate this information as completely as possible while protecting the inferential integrity of our results by avoiding multiple tests of the same thing as much as possible. In a sense, these are “our own” outcome measures; certainly the scores are on a very different scale from the raw scores. However, the factor scores are an exact weighted linear combination of all the original data, and we are confident that, in the absence of accepted scoring rules, this is the least biased way to deal statistically with the multiple correlated scores. The face validity of the factor scores is strongly argued by the fact that validated tests that purport to assess the same cognitive function do, in fact, load on the same cognitive factor. The Randts load together on factor 1, the Figural Memory scores load on factor 2, the Digit Spans load on factor 3, and the Trails B and Digit Symbol, both of which assess attention and visuospatial orientation, load on factor 4. Further validation is provided by the fact that our overall cognitive score (sum of the four factor scores) at baseline correlates very highly with a score calculated as the mean of the z scores from all tests for each individual (Pearson ρ= 0.969, Spearman ρ= 0.972).

The power to the current project needs some discussion. The cognitive factor scores are standardized in units of 1 SD. This study had at least 80% power at α= 0.05 to detect differences in change of less than half an SD on all continuous cognitive outcome measures and differences less than one tenth of an SD on the overall cognitive change score. At a significance level of 0.01, the minimum difference detectable with 80% power for the overall score is 0.105. To determine significance with a difference as small as we observed would have required a total sample size of 288,844. With respect to the less-sensitive categoric outcome, a Fisher exact test with the actual group sizes and a 0.05 two-sided significance level has 80% power to detect the difference between a warm-group proportion of deficit of 39.3% (as observed) and a cold-group proportion of deficit of 21.3%.

Several time points are used for the assessment of neurocognitive dysfunction after cardiac surgery. We chose the 6-week postoperative assessment of neurocognitive function in accord with data previously reported by our institution. 59There is a substantial reduction in measured neurocognitive deficits between discharge and testing at 4–6 weeks. Some of the markedly reduced cognitive function seen 5 to 7 days after surgery may be influenced by drugs and other factors not related to CPB and surgery. There is little difference between the incidence of cognitive dysfunction measured at the 6-week time point and 6-month or 3-yr follow-up. 59We have also demonstrated a significant association between 6-week cognitive decline, overall cognitive function, and quality of life at 5 yr. 59The stability of measured neurocognitive decline at this time point, as well as consideration of retaining patient compliance for follow-up, were the deciding factors in our selection of 6-week neurocognitive follow-up as our primary outcome.

Limitations to the current study include the fact that preoperative neurocognitive tests revealed slightly poorer preoperative performance in the cold group than the warm group. Because cognitive scores decline to a greater degree when starting at a higher level (regression toward the mean), the cold group may have been biased toward a smaller decline in overall cognitive function. However, this limitation was overcome by accounting for the baseline function in the multivariable modeling. A second limitation of our study is related to our assessment of multiple clinical neurologic and neurocognitive outcomes to determine any neuroprotective effect of hypothermic CPB. We realized that by making multiple comparisons we increased the chances of type I error occurrence. Despite this potential limitation, we did not demonstrate any neurologic and neurocognitive differences between treatment groups. A third limitation was that the patients in the cold group, rewarmed at 3–4°C difference between nasopharyngeal and CPB perfusate temperatures, were transiently exposed to central nervous system temperatures greater than 37°C during rewarming, which has been associated with an altered balance of cerebral oxygen supply and demand, leading to jugular venous desaturation and a possible increased risk of cognitive dysfunction. 30,60Cerebral hyperthermia is also known to worsen the outcome in humans after stroke by increasing the infarct size, morbidity, and mortality. 61,62Although the occurrence of hyperthermia has not been directly related to neurologic injury after CPB, it is possible that any beneficial effects of hypothermia were counteracted by the mildly hyperthermic period during rewarming. 63In our analysis, there was no association between the duration and degree of hyperthermic period and cognitive dysfunction. However, we recently found an association of rewarming rate and cognitive function. Slow rewarming rates were associated with less postoperative cognitive decline. 64Another limitation in the study is that, at 6 weeks, there was significantly lower anxiety and lower depression scores in patients randomized to normothermia. 65It could be that the lower scores in the warm group might have influenced (increased) the performance of normothermic patients in the current study. This would tend to mask any benefit in the hypothermia group. However, there was no effect of temperature randomization on overall quality of life indicators. 65Finally, as in any longitudinal study, there were subjects (n = 71) in our study who completed the baseline neuropsychologic testing but were lost at the 6-week follow-up. This loss of follow-up presents serious problems in terms of overall study design. However, the dropout rate was similar in the two treatment groups: 39 of 149 patients (26.2%) in the cold group versus  32 of 149 patients (21.5%) in the warm group (P = 0.415, Fisher exact test). Overall, the nonreturners scored significantly lower than returners on almost every measure of cognitive function, and our modeling shows that greater decline is associated with cognitive scores that start at a higher level (probably regression toward the mean). If anything, then, our nonreturners, with low initial scores, could be expected also to have low follow-up scores, but with less decline on average than the returners. Thus, it appears that bias caused by loss to follow-up has not invalidated the conclusions because the groups were so similar with respect to 6-week cognitive change, and the percent of dropouts in each group was so similar that any difference attributable to the dropouts would have to be huge to cause a significant effect.

In conclusion, we have demonstrated that a CPB regimen mimicking current practice, including temperatures of 28–30°C and obligate rewarming, does not appear to be neuroprotective. Consideration should be given to future investigation in the areas of other physiological or pharmacologic strategies for neuroprotection during cardiac surgery.

Appendix 1: Neurologic Outcome Research Group of the Duke Heart Center

Director:  Mark F. Newman, M.D.; Co-Director: James A. Blumenthal, Ph.D. Anesthesiology:  Fiona M. Clements, M.D., Norbert de Bruijn, M.D., Katherine Grichnik, M.D., Hilary P. Grocott, M.D., Steven E. Hill, M.D., Andrew K. Hilton, M.D., Joseph P. Mathew, M.D., J.G. Reves, M.D., Debra A. Schwinn, M.D., Mark Stafford Smith, M.D., David Warner, M.D., Alina M. Grigore, M.D., G. Burkhard Mackensen, M.D., Timothy Stanley, M.D., Jerry L. Kirchner, B.S., Aimee M. Butler, M.S., Vincent E. Gaver, B.A., Wayne Cohen, M.P.H., Bonita L. Funk, R.N., E.D. Derilus, B.S., Deborah Manning, BS, Scott Lee, B.S., Jonathan Williams, B.S., Melanie Tirronen, B.S., Erich Lauff, B.A., Shonna Campbell, B.S., Keinya Lee, B.S., William D. White, M.P.H., and Barbara Phillips-Bute, Ph.D. Behavioral Medicine:  James A. Blumenthal, Ph.D., Michael A. Babyak, Ph.D., and Parinda Khatri, Ph.D. Neurology:  Carmelo Graffagnino, M.D., Daniel T. Laskowitz, M.D., Ann M Saunders, Ph.D., and Warren J. Strittmatter, M.D. Surgery:  Robert W. Anderson, M.D., Thomas A. D’Amico, M.D., R. Duane Davis, M.D., Donald D. Glower, M.D., R. David Harpole, M.D., James Jaggers, M.D., Robert H. Jones, M.D., Kevin Landolfo, M.D., Carmelo Milano, M.D., Peter K. Smith, M.D., Walter G. Wolfe, M.D.

Appendix 2: Cardiothoracic Anesthesia Research Endeavors

Director:  Mark F. Newman, M.D.;Anesthesiology:  Fiona M. Clements, M.D., Norbert de Bruijn, M.D., Katherine Grichnik, M.D., Hilary P. Grocott, M.D., Steven E. Hill, M.D., Andrew K. Hilton, M.D., Joseph P. Mathew, M.D., J.G. Reves, M.D., Debra A. Schwinn, M.D., Mark Stafford Smith, M.D., Alina M. Grigore, M.D., G. Burkhard Mackensen, M.D., Timothy Stanley, M.D.

1.
Newman MF, Wolman R, Kanchuger M, Marschall K, Mora-Mangano C, Roach G, Smith LR, Aggarwal A, Nussmeier N, Herskowitz A, Mangano DT: Multicenter preoperative stroke risk index for patients undergoing coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Multicenter Study of Perioperative Ischemia (McSPI) Research Group. Circulation 1996; 94: II74–80
2.
Shaw PJ, Bates D, Cartlidge NE, French JM, Heaviside D, Julian DG, Shaw DA: Neurologic and neuropsychological morbidity following major surgery: comparison of coronary artery bypass and peripheral vascular surgery. Stroke 1987; 18: 700–7
3.
Murkin JM, Martzke JS, Buchan AM, Bentley C, Wong CJ: A randomized study of the influence of perfusion technique and pH management strategy in 316 patients undergoing coronary artery bypass surgery. II. Neurologic and cognitive outcomes. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 1995; 110: 349–62
4.
Peterson ED, Cowper PA, Jollis JG, Bebchuk JD, DeLong ER, Muhlbaier LH, Mark DB, Pryor DB: Outcomes of coronary artery bypass graft surgery in 24,461 patients aged 80 years or older. Circulation 1995; 92: II85–91
5.
Jones EL, Weintraub WS, Carver JM, Guyton RA, Cohen CL: Cornary bypass surgery: Is the operation different today? J Thorac Cardiovacs Surg 1991; 101: 108–15
6.
Mohan R, Amsel BJ, Walter PL: Coronary artery bypass grafting in the elderly: A review of studies on patients older than 64, 69, or 74 years. Cardiology 1992; 80: 215–25
7.
Cosgrove DM, Loop FD, Lytle BW, Baillot R, Gill CC, Golding LA, Taylor PC, Goormastic M: Primary myocardial revascularization: Trends in surgical mortality. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 1984; 88: 673–84
8.
Menasche P, Peynet J, Touchot B, Aziz M, Haydar S, Perez G, Veyssie L, Montenegro J, Bloch G, Piwnica A: Normothermic cardioplegia: Is aortic cross-clamping still synonymous with myocardial ischemia? Ann Thorac Surg 1992; 54: 472–7
9.
Christakis GT, Koch JP, Deemar KA, Fremes SE, Sinclair L, Chen E, Salerno TA, Goldman BS, Lichtenstein SV: A randomized study of the systemic effects of warm heart surgery. Ann Thorac Surg 1992; 54: 449–57
10.
Kavanagh BP, Mazer CD, Panos A, Lichtenstein SV: Effect of warm heart surgery on perioperative management of patients undergoing urgent cardiac surgery. J Cardiothorac Vasc Anesth 1992; 6: 127–31
11.
Newman MF, Croughwell ND, Blumenthal JA, White WD, Lewis JB, Smith LR, Frasco PF, Towner EA, Schell RM, Hurwitz BJ, Reves JG: The effect of aging on cerebral autoregulation during cardiopulmonary bypass: Association with postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Circulation 1994; 90: 243–9
12.
Wechsler D: The Wechsler Adult Inteligence Scale-Revised (Manual). San Antonio, Psychological Corporation, 1981
13.
Theil DR, Stanley TE, White WD, Goodman DK, Glass PS, Bai SA, Jacobs JR, Reves JG: Midazolam and fentanyl continuous infusion anesthesia for cardiac surgery: A comparison of computer-assisted versus manual infusion systems. J Cardiothorac Vasc Anesth 1993; 7: 300–6
14.
Cattell RB: The Scientific Use of Factor Analysis in Behavioral and Life Sciences. New York, Plenum, 1978, pp 1–618
15.
Morrison DF: Multivariate Statistical Methods, 2nd edition. New York, McGraw-Hill, 1976, pp 266–343
16.
Buckberg GD, Brazier JR, Nelson RL, Goldstein SM, McConnell DH, Cooper N: Studies of the effects of hypothermia on regional myocardial blood flow and metabolism during cardiopulmonary bypass. I. The adequately perfused beating, fibrillating, and arrested heart. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 1977; 73: 87–94
17.
Michenfelder JD, Milde JH: The relationship among canine brain temperature, metabolism, and function during hypothermia. A nesthesiology 1991; 75: 130–6
18.
Busto R, Globus MY, Dietrich WD, Martinez E, Valdes I, Ginsberg MD: Effect of mild hypothermia on ischemia-induced release of neurotransmitters and free fatty acids in rat brain. Stroke 1989; 20: 904–10
19.
Bickler PE, Buck LT, Hansen BM: Effects of isoflurane and hypothermia on glutamate receptor-mediated calcium influx in brain slices. A nesthesiology 1994; 81: 1461–9
20.
Widmann R, Miyazawa T, Hossmann KA: Protective effect of hypothermia on hippocampal injury after 30 minutes of forebrain ischemia in rats is mediated by postischemic recovery of protein synthesis. J Neurochem 1993; 61: 200–9
21.
Busto R, Globus MY, Neary JT, Ginsberg MD: Regional alterations of protein kinase C activity following transient cerebral ischemia: Effects of intraischemic brain temperature modulation. J Neurochem 1994; 63: 1095–103
22.
Nakashima K, Todd MM, Warner DS: The relation between cerebral metabolic rate and ischemic depolarization: A comparison of the effects of hypothermia, pentobarbital, and isoflurane. A nesthesiology 1995; 82: 1199–208
23.
Globus MY, Busto R, Lin B, Schnippering H, Ginsberg MD: Detection of free radical activity during transient global ischemia and recirculation: Effects of intraischemic brain temperature modulation. J Neurochem 1995; 65: 1250–6
24.
Kader A, Frazzini VI, Baker CJ, Solomon RA, Trifiletti RR: Effect of mild hypothermia on nitric oxide synthesis during focal cerebral ischemia. Neurosurgery 1994; 35: 272–7
25.
Chen Q, Chopp M, Bodzin G, Chen H: Temperature modulation of cerebral depolarization during focal cerebral ischemia in rats: Correlation with ischemic injury. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1993; 13: 389–94
26.
Hindman BJ, Todd MM, Gelb AW, Loftus CM, Craen RA, Schubert A, Mahla ME, Torner JC: Mild hypothermia as a protective therapy during intracranial aneurysm surgery: A randomized prospective pilot trial. Neurosurgery 1999; 44: 23–32
27.
Marion DW, Penrod LE, Kelsey SF, Obrist WD, Kochanek PM, Palmer AM, Wisniewski SR, DeKosky ST: Treatment of traumatic brain injury with moderate hypothermia. N Engl J Med 1997; 336: 540–6
28.
Magovern GJ Jr, Flaherty JT, Gott VL, Bulkley BH, Gardner TJ: Failure of blood cardioplegia to protect myocardium at lower temperatures. Circulation 1982; 66: I60–7
29.
Grocott HP, Newman MF, Croughwell ND, White WD, Lowry E, Reves JG: Continuous jugular venous versus nasopharyngeal temperature monitoring during hypothermic cardiopulmonary bypass for cardiac surgery. J Clin Anesth 1997; 9: 312–6
30.
Cook DJ, Orszulak TA, Daly RC, Buda DA: Cerebral hyperthermia during cardiopulmonary bypass in adults. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 1996; 111: 268–9
31.
Croughwell ND, Frasco P, Blumenthal JA, Leone BJ, White WD, Reves JG: Warming during cardiopulmonary bypass is associated with jugular bulb desaturation. Ann Thorac Surg 1992; 53: 827–32
32.
Cook DJ, Oliver WC Jr, Orszulak TA, Daly RC: A prospective, randomized comparison of cerebral venous oxygen saturation during normothermic and hypothermic cardiopulmonary bypass. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 1994; 107: 1020–8
33.
Noback CR, Tinker JH: Hypothermia after cardiopulmonary bypass in man: Amelioration by nitroprusside-induced vasodilation during rewarming. A nesthesiology 1980; 53: 277–80
34.
Schmied H, Kurz A, Sessler DI, Kozek S, Reiter A: Mild hypothermia increases blood loss and transfusion requirements during total hip arthroplasty. Lancet 1996; 347: 289–92
35.
Valeri CR, Feingold H, Cassidy G, Ragno G, Khuri S, Altschule MD: Hypothermia-induced reversible platelet dysfunction. Ann Surg 1987; 205: 175–81
36.
Kurz A, Sessler DI, Lenhardt R: Perioperative normothermia to reduce the incidence of surgical-wound infection and shorten hospitalization: Study of Wound Infection and Temperature Group. N Engl J Med 1996; 334: 1209–15
37.
Sessler DI, Rubinstein EH, Moayeri A: Physiologic responses to mild perianesthetic hypothermia in humans. A nesthesiology 1991; 75: 594–610
38.
Lichtenstein SV, Ashe KA, el Dalati H, Cusimano RJ, Panos A, Slutsky AS: Warm heart surgery. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 1991; 101: 269–74
39.
Martin TD, Craver JM, Gott JP, Weintraub WS, Ramsay J, Mora CT, Guyton RA: Prospective, randomized trial of retrograde warm blood cardioplegia: Myocardial benefit and neurologic threat. Ann Thorac Surg 1994; 57: 298–302
40.
Mora CT, Henson MB, Weintraub WS, Murkin JM, Martin TD, Craver JM, Gott JP, Guyton RA: The effect of temperature management during cardiopulmonary bypass on neurologic and neuropsychologic outcomes in patients undergoing coronary revascularization. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 1996; 112: 514–22
41.
Engelman RM, Pleet AB, Rousou JA, Flack JE 3rd, Deaton DW, Pekow PS, Gregory CA: Influence of cardiopulmonary bypass perfusion temperature on neurologic and hematologic function after coronary artery bypass grafting. Ann Thorac Surg 1999; 67: 1547–55
42.
McLean RF, Wong BI, Naylor CD, Snow WG, Harrington EM, Gawel M, Fremes SE: Cardiopulmonary bypass, temperature, and central nervous system dysfunction. Circulation 1994; 90: II250–5
43.
Plourde G, Leduc AS, Morin JE, DeVarennes B, Latter D, Symes J, Robbins R, Fosset N, Couture L, Ptito A: Temperature during cardiopulmonary bypass for coronary artery operations does not influence postoperative cognitive function: A prospective, randomized trial. J Thorac Cardiovasc Surg 1997; 114: 123–8
44.
Singh AK, Bert AA, Feng WC, Rotenberg FA: Stroke during coronary artery bypass grafting using hypothermic versus normothermic perfusion. Ann Thorac Surg 1995; 59: 84–9
45.
The Warm Heart Investigators: Randomised trial of normothermic versus hypothermic coronary bypass surgery. Lancet 1994; 343: 559–63
The Warm Heart Investigators:
46.
Santacruz KS, Swagerty D: Early diagnosis of dementia. Am Fam Physician 2001; 63: 703–13
47.
Elias MF, Beiser A, Wolf PA, Au R, White RF, D’Agostino RB: The preclinical phase of Alzheimer disease: A 22-year prospective study of the Framingham Cohort. Arch Neurol 2000; 57: 808–13
48.
Geldmacher DS, Whitehouse PJ Jr: Differential diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1997; 48: S2–9
49.
Jehkonen M, Ahonen JP, Dastidar P, Laippala P, Vilkki J: Unawareness of deficits after right hemisphere stroke: Double-dissociations of anosognosias. Acta Neurologica Scandinavia 2000; 102: 378–84
50.
Schut LJ: Dementia following stroke. Clin Geriatr Med 1988; 4: 767–84
51.
Doody RS, Geldmacher DS, Gordon B, Perdomo CA, Pratt RD, Group DS: Open-label, multicenter, phase 3 extension study of the safety and efficacy of donepezil in patients with Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 2001; 58: 427–33
Group DS
52.
Broderick JP, Gaskill M, Dhawan A, Khoury JC: Temporal changes in brain volume and cognition in a randomized treatment trial of vascular dementia. Neuroimaging 2001; 11: 6–12
53.
Murkin JM, Newman SP, Stump DA, Blumenthal JA: Statement of consensus on assessment of neurobehavioral outcomes after cardiac surgery. Ann Thorac Surg 1995; 59: 1289–95
54.
Murkin JM, Stump DA, Blumenthal JA, McKhann G: Defining dysfunction: Group means versus incidence analysis—A statement of consensus. Ann Thorac Surg 1997; 64: 904–5
55.
Murkin JM, Boyd WD, Ganapathy S, Adams SJ, Peterson RC: Beating heart surgery: Why expect less central nervous system morbidity? Ann Thorac Surg 1999; 68: 1498–501
56.
Mitchell SJ, Pellett O, Gorman DF: Cerebral protection by lidocaine during cardiac operations. Ann Thorac Surg 1999; 67: 1117–24
57.
Butterworth J, Legault C, Stump DA, Coker L, Hammon JW, Troost BT, Royster RI, Prough DS: A randomized, blinded trial of the antioxidant pegorgotein: No reduction in neuropsychological deficits, inotropic drug support, or myocardial ischemia after coronary artery bypass. J Cardiothoracic Vasc Anesth 1999; 13: 690–4
58.
Newman MF, Grocott HP, Stanley TO, Mackensen GB, Kirchner JL, Mark DB, Blumenthal JA: Neurocognitive dysfunction and quality-of-life after cardiac surgery. Anesth Analg 2000; 90: SCA4
59.
Newman MF, Kirchner J, Phillips-Bute B, Phillips E, Han D, Baudet B, Grigore AM, Reves JG, Blumenthal J: Perioperative neurocognitive decline predicts long-term neurocognitive deterioration after CABG. Anesth Analg 1999; 88: SCA93
60.
Croughwell ND, Newman MF, Blumenthal JA, White WD, Lewis JB, Frasco PE, Smith LR, Thyrum EA, Hurwitz BJ, Leone BJ, RM S, JG R: Jugular bulb saturation and cognitive dysfunction after cardiopulmonary bypass. Ann Thorac Surg 1994; 58: 1702–8
61.
Azzimondi G, Bassein L, Nonino F, Fiorani L, Vignatelli L, Re G, D’Alessandro R: Fever in acute stroke worsens prognosis: A prospective study. Stroke 1995; 26: 2040–3
62.
Reith J, Jorgensen HS, Pedersen PM, Nakayama H, Raaschou HO, Jeppesen LL, Olsen TS: Body temperature in acute stroke: Relation to stroke severity, infarct size, mortality, and outcome. Lancet 1996; 347: 422–5
63.
Grigore AM, Grocott HP, Mathew JP, Phillips-Bute B, Stanley TO, Butler A, Reves JG, Blumenthal JA, Newman MF: The association of rewarming rate and neurocognitive dysfunction after cardiac surgery. Anesth Analg 2000; 90: SCA34
64.
Newman MF, Kramer D, Croughwell ND, Sanderson I, Blumenthal JA, White WD, Smith LR, Towner EA, Reves JG: Differential age effects of mean arterial pressure and rewarming on cognitive dysfunction after cardiac surgery. Anesth Analg 1995; 81: 236–42
65.
Khatri P, Babyak M, Croughwell ND, Davis R, White WD, Newman MF, Reves JG, Mark DB, Blumenthal JA: Temperature during coronary artery bypass surgery affects quality of life. Ann Thorac Surg 2001; 71: 110–6